Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
To err is human: Patient misinterpretations of prescription
Michael S. Wolf ,Terry C. Davis William Shrank David N. Rapp ,,
Pat F. Bass , Ulla M. Connor Marla Clayman , Ruth M. Parker
a Health Literacy and Learning Program, Institute for Healthcare Studies, Northwestern University, United States
b Center for Communication and Medicine, Division of General Internal Medicine, Northwestern University, United States
c Louisiana State University Health Sciences Center at Shreveport, United States
d Brigham and Women’s Hospital and Harvard Medical School, Boston, MA, United States
e School of Education and Social Policy, Northwestern University, United States
f Department of Psychology, Northwestern University, United States
g Indiana Center for Intercultural Communication, Indiana University-Purdue University at Indianapolis, United States
h Emory University School of Medicine, United States
Received 23 February 2007; received in revised form 28 March 2007; accepted 30 March 2007
Objective: To examine the nature and cause of patients’ misunderstanding common dosage instructions on prescription drug container labels. Methods: In-person cognitive interviews including a literacy assessment were conducted among 395 patients at one of three primary care clinics inShreveport, Louisiana, Jackson, Michigan and Chicago, Illinois. Patients were asked to read and demonstrate understanding of dosage instructionsfor five common prescription medications. Correct understanding was determined by a panel of blinded physician raters reviewing patient verbatimresponses. Qualitative methods were employed to code incorrect responses and generate themes regarding causes for misunderstanding. Results: Rates of misunderstanding for the five dosage instructions ranged from 8 to 33%. Patients with low literacy had higher rates ofmisunderstanding compared to those with marginal or adequate literacy (63% versus 51% versus 38%, p < 0.001). The 374 (19%) incorrectresponses were qualitatively reviewed. Six themes were derived to describe the common causes for misunderstanding: label language, complexityof instructions, implicit versus explicit dosage intervals, presence of distractors, label familiarity, and attentiveness to label instructions. Conclusion: Misunderstanding dosage instructions on prescription drug labels is common. While limited literacy is associated with misunder-standing, the instructions themselves are awkwardly phrased, vague, and unnecessarily difficult. Practice implications: Prescription drug labels should use explicit dosing intervals, clear and simple language, within a patient-friendly labelformat. Health literacy and cognitive factors research should be consulted. # 2007 Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
Keywords: Prescription; Drug; Medication; Dosage; Instructions; Warnings; Misunderstanding; Health literacy
prescription drug labeling were specifically cited as a leadingroot cause of a large proportion of outpatient medication errors
According to the Institute of Medicine (IOM) 2006 report,
and adverse events, as patients may unintentionally misuse a
Preventing Medication Error, more than one third of the 1.5
prescribed medicine due to improper understanding of
million adverse drug events that occur in the United States each
instructions. The prescription container label, in particular,
year happen in outpatient settings . Problems with
is often the sole, tangible source of specific dosage/usageinstructions given to and repeatedly used by the patient. Despite their potential value, problems are clearly evident with
* Corresponding author at: Health Literacy and Learning Program, Institute
container labels Dosage instructions on the label can
for Healthcare Studies and Division of General Internal Medicine, North-
vary, as they are dependent on what the prescribing physician
western University, 676 N. St. Clair Street, Suite 200, Chicago, IL 60611,
writes, as well as how the pharmacist interprets them .
United States. Tel.: +1 312 695 0459; fax: +1 312 695 4307.
With little guidance available to providers, instructions
0738-3991/$ – see front matter # 2007 Published by Elsevier Ireland Ltd.
M.S. Wolf et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
commonly found on prescription drug labels may not
always be clearly stated. In prior studies, half of adults inoutpatient primary care settings misunderstood one or more
The methods and quantitative findings from this research
primary and auxiliary prescription instructions and warnings
study that detail the relationship between patient literacy and
they encountered Patients with limited literacy skills
misunderstanding prescription label instructions have been
and those managing multiple medication regimens made more
Improving prescription drug container label instructions is
both a matter of health literacy and patient safety . This isespecially true since other sources of patient medication
Subjects were adult patients who attended one of three
information are insufficient. Prior studies have found that
outpatient primary care clinics serving low-income community
physicians and pharmacists frequently miss opportunities to
populations in Shreveport, Louisiana, Jackson, Michigan and
adequately counsel patients on newly prescribed medicines
Chicago, Illinois. Recruitment took place over consecutive
Other supplementary sources, such as consumer
summers beginning July 2003. Patients were eligible if they
medication information sheets and FDA-approved medication
were 18 or older, and ineligible if the clinic nurse or study
guides that may be dispensed with a prescribed medicine are
research assistant identified a patient as having one or more of
too complex and written at a reading grade level too high for the
the following conditions: (1) blindness or severely impaired
majority of patients to comprehend . As a result, these
vision not correctable with eyeglasses; (2) deafness or hearing
materials are not read Patients’ ability to decipher the
problems uncorrectable with a hearing aid; (3) too ill to
brief text instructions on the container label itself takes on
participate; (4) non-English speaking. Institutional Review
greater importance to ensure proper use.
Boards at each location approved the study.
A total of 458 patients were approached in the order they
1.1. Sources of comprehension failure: a conceptual model
arrived at the clinics and prior to the medical encounter. Twelvepatients refused participation 26 were deemed ineligible, and
The ability to read and understand prescription label
25 had incomplete information, leaving 395 patients participat-
instructions may appear to be a simple task, yet van den
ing in the study. A response rate was determined following
Broek & Kremer describe various sources of failure in
American Association for Public Opinion Research (AAPOR)
comprehension that are particularly applicable for the
standards; 92% of approached eligible patients participated in
abbreviated text on container labels These include
readers’ cognitive characteristics, constraints on the readingsituation, and the nature of the presented health information.
2.2. Structured interview and literacy assessment
The influence of the latter set of factors is particularlyapplicable to the truncated text on container labels, and may
A structured, cognitive interview protocol was developed to
include text complexity, formatting and organizational issues.
assess understanding of different label dosage instructions
Failure may also occur if instructions are not explicit, or if
placed on five common prescription medications. This process
purpose is not evident, such as providing an indication for use
has been widely used by the research team, among others
on the bottle label itself (i.e. ‘‘take for diabetes’’), which is not
These included two antibiotics (amoxicillin (for
part of routine practice for either physicians to add to the script
pediatric use) and trimethoprim), an expectorant (guaifenesin),
or pharmacists to include on the dispensed container label. The
an anti-hypertensive, channel blocking agent (felodipine), and a
presence of distracting information may limit comprehension
diuretic (furosemide). A trained research assistant (RA) at each
of the pertinent dosage/usage instructions and auxiliary
site administered the interview to consenting patients that
warnings that patients need to understand in order to safely
included self-report of sociodemographic information (age,
use a medicine. This might include the more prominently
gender, race/ethnicity, education) source of payment for
displayed pharmacy logo, phone number, serial number and
medications, and number of prescription medications currently
drug code, and other provider-directed content on the label.
taken daily. Actual prescription pill bottle containers withlabels were then shown to patients, one at a time, for review.
Once patients provided their interpretation on all of the labels,the RA administered a brief literacy assessment, ending the
The purpose of this study was to investigate how patients
approached and interpreted prescription drug label instructions,and to document the nature of misunderstanding that may
2.2.1. Understanding of medication primary container
contribute to the high prevalence of medication error. We took a
health literacy perspective towards the problem of misunder-
To assess patient understanding of prescription medication
standing prescription medication instructions. From this view,
instructions included on the container primary labels, the RA
it was hypothesized that misunderstanding would be the result
asked ‘‘how would you take this medicine?’’ This question was
of both patient literacy limitations and the ambiguity and
often followed by one to two short probes (i.e. ‘‘anything
inherent difficulty of label instructions themselves.
else?’’, ‘‘exactly how would you take the pills [medicine]?’’) to
M.S. Wolf et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
initiate more detailed description of administration. The RA
documented the verbatim response on a separate form. Responses to the instructions for the five medications
Mixed methods were used. Chi-square tests were calculated
(N = 1,975) were then independently rated correct or incorrect
to examine bivariate associations between health literacy
by three general internal medicine attending physicians from
(adequate, marginal, low), sociodemographic variables (age,
three different academic medical centers. Each physician rater
gender, race, education, number of medications currently
was blinded to all patient information and was trained to follow
taken), and understanding (yes or no) primary label instructions
stringent coding guidelines agreed upon previously by the
and attendance (yes or no) to the auxiliary warning instructions.
research team. Correct scores were to be given only if patient
Quantitative analyses were conducted using Stata 9.0 (College
responses included all aspects of the label’s instruction,
including dosage, timing, and if applicable, duration.
For qualitative analyses, a grounded theory approach was
Inter-rater reliability was high (k = 0.85). The 147 responses
used to explore the basis for patients’ misunderstanding of
(7.4%) that received discordant ratings between the three
each of the five dosage instructions using their documented
reviewers were sent to an expert panel that included three primary
verbatim responses . Grounded theory is a systematic
care physicians and two health literacy experts for further review.
method for generating theoretical statements from case
Each panel member, also blinded to patient information,
studies. Based on our qualitative, cognitive interviews,
independently reviewed and coded responses as correct or
grounded theory guides the inductive process of organizing
incorrect. For 76.2 % (n = 112) of the 147 responses, consensus
content derived from patient responses. Patient misunder-
was achieved among the five-member panel. A majority rule was
standings were first reviewed by investigators (MSW, TCD,
imposed for the remaining responses (n = 35).
RMP) and classified using both selective and in vivo codingschemes . Data were then reduced by one of the lead
2.2.2. Attendance to auxiliary (secondary) warning label
investigators (MSW) through detailed a priori coding to
classify the reason for error in understanding (label language,
Attentiveness to the auxiliary or ‘‘secondary’’ warning label
complexity, explicitness of instruction, presence of distracters,
on the pill bottles by patients was also investigated. These
and label familiarity). These predetermined codes were based
labels provide supplementary instructions supporting the safe
on previous studies and the conceptual model of sources of
administration of the medications, such as ‘‘take with food’’ or
comprehension failure . The reduced data was confirmed
‘‘do not chew or crush, swallow whole.’’ RAs were instructed
based on the a priori coding scheme, and in vivo codes were
during the interview to document (yes or no) whether patients
allowed to develop based on emergent themes in responses.
either attempted to interpret the auxiliary label along with the
Agreement among investigators was sought prior to classifying
primary label, or physically turned the bottle to inspect the
patient responses with any new themes. Open coding
color stickers on which these warning messages are placed.
techniques were used . Qualitative analyses weresupported by NVivo 7 software (QSR International; Doncaster,
2.2.3. Reading versus demonstrating instructions
Patients were further tested on their functional under-
standing of the primary label instruction for guaifenesin (‘‘take
two tablets by mouth twice daily’’). They were asked todemonstrate how many pills were to be taken on a daily basis.
After patients answered the first question, ‘‘how would you takethis medicine?’’ they were asked, ‘‘show me how many pills
describes the study sample in detail, stratified by
you would take [of this medicine] in one day’’. The container
literacy. The mean age was 45 years (S.D. = 14; range 19–85
was filled with candy pills for patients to dispense and count out
years). Fifty-seven percent of patients were recruited from
the correct amount. Responses were coded as correct if their
Shreveport, Louisiana, 25% from Jackson, Michigan, and 18%
answer was ‘‘four’’, and incorrect if any other response was
from Chicago, Illinois. Two-thirds (68%) were female,
approximately half of patients were African American (47%)and half white (48%), and 28% reported less than a high school
level of education attainment. Patient literacy was limited; 19%
Patient literacy was assessed using the Rapid Estimate of
were reading at or below a sixth grade level (low literacy) and
Adult Literacy in Medicine (REALM), a reading recognition
29% were reading at the seventh to eighth grade level (marginal
test comprised of 66 health-related words . The REALM is
the most commonly used test of patient literacy in medical
Patients were taking an average of three prescription
settings . In healthcare studies where patients need only be
medications, and 23% lacked insurance to cover these
categorized as low (scores 0–44), marginal (scores 45–60) or
prescribed drugs. The physician was the most likely source
adequate (scores 61–66) readers, the information provided by
of medication information for patients (71%). Low literacy was
the REALM is generally sufficient. The REALM is highly
associated with older age ( p < 0.001), African American
correlated with standardized reading tests and the Test of
race ( p < 0.001), and less education ( p < 0.001); differences
Functional Health Literacy in Adults (TOFHLA) .
were also noted by site ( p < 0.002). No significant differences
M.S. Wolf et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
Table 1Sample characteristics stratified by literacy level
Source of support for understanding prescription medication instructions (%)
Number of medications taken daily, mean (S.D.)
Misunderstanding 1 or more dosage instructions (%)
were reported between literacy, gender, source of payment for
versus 80%, p < 0.001). No statistically significant associations
medications, or number of prescription medications taken daily.
were noted by number of medications or age.
3.2. Prevalence and associations of misunderstanding
3.4. Nature of patient misunderstanding label dosage
Overall, 46% of patients misunderstood one or more dosage
The 374 (18.9%) total responses that were coded as incorrect
instructions. The prevalence of misunderstanding among
were qualitatively reviewed and coded using the pre-selected
patients with adequate, marginal and low literacy was 38%,51%, and 63%, respectively ( p < 0.001). The rates ofmisunderstanding individual labels ranged from 8% for theinstructions on the label for Felodipine (‘‘Take one tablet bymouth once each day’’) to 33% for the instructions for Trime-thoprim (‘‘Take one tablet by mouth twice daily for 7 days’’;). Patients with low literacy were less able to understandinstructions compared to those with adequate literacy.
3.3. Reading versus demonstrating dosage instructions
The ability to read dosage instructions did not always
preclude the ability to demonstrate a functional understandingof prescription drug use (When asked how pills were tobe taken in a given day for the instruction, ‘‘Take two tablets bymouth twice daily’’, one third of patients were unable tocorrectly state ‘‘four pills’’. Rather, the most common incorrectanswer was ‘‘two pills’’. Patients with low literacy were lessable to state the correct number of pills taken daily compared to
Fig. 1. Rates of correct understanding vs. Demonstration for the primary label
those with marginal and adequate literacy (35% versus 63%
instruction, ‘‘Take two tablets by mouth twice daily’’.
M.S. Wolf et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
Table 2Rates of understanding primary label instructions and attendance to auxiliary warnings, stratified by literacy level
Take one teaspoonful by mouth three times daily
Refrigerate, shake well, discard after [date]
Take one tablet by mouth twice daily for 7 days
You should avoid prolonged or excessive exposure
to direct and/or artificial sunlight while taking this medication
Medication should be taken with plenty of water
Take one tablet in the morning and one at 5 p.m.
Do not take dairy products, antacids, or iron preparations
a Included behavioral demonstration for Guaifenesin only.
coding scheme of likely causes for error in interpretation
(n = 75), 79% of these patients could not recognize and
(). One emergent cause, referred to as attentiveness to
pronounce ‘‘antibiotic’’, 73% ‘‘orally’’, 70% ‘‘teaspoonful’’,
label instructions, was included in addition to the predeter-
48% ‘‘medication’’, 45% ‘‘prescription’’, and 35% the word
mined causes of label language, complexity of instructions,
‘‘dose’’. Poor word recognition may have contributed to
implicit versus explicit dosage, presence of distracters, and
patients misreading words on labels, such as ‘‘tablespoon’’
instead of ‘‘teaspoon’’. This accounted for 9% of errors(n = 34).
Interestingly, feedback documented by RAs from patient
Certain common phrases used on medicine labels seemed
interviews recommended the use of numeric symbols within the
confusing and unfamiliar to patients within the context of the
instruction rather than the written word equivalent (i.e. ‘‘2’’
instruction itself. Errors that appeared to be the result of label
versus ‘‘two’’) for further reading ease.
language were most prevalent on the instruction, ‘‘Take twotablets by mouth twice daily’’. The repetitiveness between
dosage (‘‘two’’) and frequency (‘‘twice’’) often led to the
Instructions ranged in complexity, both with regards to the
common interpretation ‘‘Take a pill twice a day’’, whereas
calculation of the number of pills and times to be taken daily
dosage would go ignored. This was confirmed in the follow-up
(i.e. ‘‘Take one pill by mouth once each day’’ versus, ‘‘Take two
demonstration task, ‘‘How many pills would you take in one
tablets by mouth twice daily’’) and in the amount of content to
day’’ with the common incorrect response of ‘‘two’’ (72% of
be retained (dosage, frequency, and/or duration, as in ‘‘Take one
tablet by mouth twice daily for seven days’’). Patients found
Many terms commonly used on prescription labels had
simpler dosing regimens to be easier to understand, while more
exceptionally poor recognition rates by patients. Specifically,
complex regimens had more errors in their interpretation
among patients reading at the 6th grade level and below
). Eleven percent (n = 41) of incorrect responses
Table 3Examples of the most common misunderstandings, by dosage instruction
Take one teaspoonful by mouth three times daily
Take three teaspoons daily; take three tablespoons every day; you should drink it three times a day
Take one tablet by mouth twice daily for 7 days
Take two pills a day; take it for 7 days; take one every day for a week; I’d take a pill everyday for 7 days
Take it every 8 h; take it every day; take one every 12 h
Take one tablet in the morning and one at 5 p.m.
I would take it every day at 5 o’clock; take it at 5 p.m.
M.S. Wolf et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
omitted duration of use from the specified instruction. The
Mistaking ‘‘teaspoon’’ for ‘‘tablespoon’’ was more common
inclusion of duration on the label instruction also led to a loss of
among patients with limited literacy, but one third of these
other aspects of the instruction. For the label, ‘‘Take one tablet
errors were made by patients with adequate literacy (n = 12).
by mouth twice daily for seven days’’, the second most commonerror made was an incorrect interpretation of dosing frequency
(n = 34; i.e. ‘‘I’d take a pill every day for seven days’’).
3.4.3. Implicit versus explicit dosage intervals
Patients were better able to interpret more explicit dose
Although there may be a finite number of ways a physician
frequencies as in ‘‘Take one tablet in the morning and one at 5
can prescribe a medicine, the same dose and frequency
p.m.’’ (90%), compared with the more vague ‘‘Take two tablets
schedule may be written in several different ways (i.e. every
by mouth twice daily’’ (83%), and ‘‘Take one teaspoonful by
12 h, twice daily, in the morning and evening, 8 a.m. and 5 p.m.,
mouth three times daily’’ (73%). For the latter two instructions,
etc.). This becomes especially problematic as many patients
patients varied in their interpretation of ‘‘twice daily’’ and
may have more than one healthcare provider prescribing
‘‘three times daily’’. For example, patients interpreted ‘‘twice
medicine . The ability to follow instructions is crucial in
daily’’ as both ‘‘every 8 h’’ and ‘‘every 12 h’’, and ‘‘three times
ambulatory care, since the patient assumes the bulk of
daily’’ ranged from ‘‘every 4 h’’ to ‘‘every 8 h’’.
responsibility for medication safety. Our present researchoffers timely evidence classifying the nature and causes of
In 6% (n = 21) of the incorrect responses, patients had
instructions that could potentially lead to errors and adverse
difficulty navigating the label content itself and identifying the
instructional content. Rather than describing the dosage of the
Our prior studies have repeatedly shown that limited literacy
medicine, responses detailed provider-directed content that
significantly impairs one’s ability to read and demonstrate an
surrounded and may have obscured the dosage instructions (i.e.
understanding of instructions and warnings found on com-
stated combinations for the name of the drug, physician’s name,
monly prescribed medicines While individual differ-
refill and date). Patients turned the bottle to acknowledge
ences in reading ability may be related to a greater risk for
auxiliary warnings, as they were also recited along with the
misunderstanding, problems are clearly evident with the label
provider-directed content instead of the dose and frequency for
itself, and the implicit nature and syntax of instructions.
use (i.e. ‘‘Take it with Food’’; ‘‘I would take them every day but
Improving the reading ease of dosage instructions is therefore
not with dairy products, antacids, or iron preparations’’; ‘‘I
Many patients might presume the task of reviewing
prescription drug labels to be overly simple. As a result, they
may not allot adequate time to process and understand the
Auxiliary instructions are often placed as stickers surround-
information. This could explain why a majority of patients were
ing or in back of the primary label. Very few patients were
able to read back the instruction, while far fewer could
familiar with these instructions. Less than 10% of patients
demonstrate a proper understanding when probed further. An
physically turned any of the bottles to examine these stickers
earlier study by Morrell and colleagues found that older adults,
(). Sixteen percent of patients attended to at least one
who on average manage more medications than younger
auxiliary instruction, and 2% made the action part of the routine
patients, spent less time processing dosage instructions and
inspection of the prescription bottle for all five medicines.
consequently made more errors in interpretation . Thesemistakes could lead to compromised health outcomes, such as
3.4.6. Attentiveness to label instructions
under-treatment (i.e. taking two rather than four pills a day) or
Several patients provided detailed responses that verbally
possible harm (i.e. taking too much of a medicine or not
‘implemented’ the regimen (‘‘It’s an antibiotic, and I would
take one pill in the morning when I wake, and another pill after
The manner in which physicians write dosage instructions
dinner—I would do that for a week’’). Even though tasks were
requires patients to make inferences as to when to specifically
not timed, many patients appeared to have responded quickly,
implement the prescribed regimen (i.e. Take two tablets by
and by doing so made simple mistakes. When answers were
mouth twice daily; Take one teaspoon by mouth three times
provided in haste, patients often skipped or omitted dosage
daily). Our findings suggest that patients’ interpretations may
information (‘‘Take two a day’’; ‘‘I’d take three pills daily’’).
widely vary when dosing intervals are presented in vague terms
Patients with adequate literacy were more likely than
as ‘‘twice daily’’ or ‘‘three times daily’’, which may stray from
patients with low literacy to omit the duration of use for the
the original intent of the prescribing physician. Park and
instruction, ‘‘Take one tablet by mouth twice daily for seven
colleagues suggest that making inferences is a complex
days’’ (n = 41; 44% versus 18%, p < 0.001). They were equally
cognitive process, and the elderly may have greater difficulty
likely to make errors wherein dose and interval were inverted
for the same instruction and for ‘‘Take one teaspoonful by
Some misunderstandings appeared to be the result of
mouth three times daily’’ (n = 60; 39% versus 43%, p = 0.65).
container label organization. The prescription labels were
M.S. Wolf et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
typical of the order in which most pharmacies present drug
4.4.1. Use explicit language when describing dose
information, often emphasizing (by yellow highlight, large
font, bold text) content that is irrelevant to the patient. The
Three previous studies also found more explicit instruction
inclusion of such distracting information may be particularly
improved comprehension . This might help pace
problematic for individuals with limited literacy, who face
patients and allow them to direct necessary attention for
greater reading difficulty in less familiar and technical contexts
processing each component of dosage. For instance, the actual
dose (number of pills to be taken at a time) could be separatedfrom the interval (times per day), as in the example ‘‘Take 2
tablets in the morning, and take 2 tablets in the evening.’’
We investigated patient understanding of prescription drug
4.4.2. Organize label in a way to minimize distracters
label instructions, not whether a medication error occurred.
The label should be re-organized, separating distracting
Patients’ actual prescription drug-taking behaviors were not
elements that often comprise provider-directed content
examined. Patients’ motivation, concentration and comprehen-
(pharmacy logo, drug serial number, pharmacy address and
sion might have been greater if they reported on their own
phone number) away from dosage instructions. Auxiliary
medicine. Similarly, we interviewed patients before their
instructions might also be placed in a set location (i.e. backside
medical encounter. It is also possible that the reason for the
of label), instead of being stuck on in various locations, so
medical visit altered patients’ concentration, although patients
patients can have routine expectations of their location.
were offered the opportunity to refuse and anyone too acutelyill was not interviewed. We also did not conduct a chart review,
and thereby could not identify if patients had actual experience
Doak, Doak, and Root (1993) offer guidance as to how to
with any of the study medications. Only patients who were
make health information more suitable for patients with limited
proficient in the English language were also included. This was
literacy, such as dosage instructions and warning messages on
due in part to criteria for using the Rapid Estimate of Adult
auxiliary labels. The use of numbers rather than the text
Literacy in Medicine (REALM) as our literacy assessment.
equivalent should be promoted for reading ease, and unclarified
Further research should investigate the effect of cultural
medical jargon (i.e. antibiotic) or awkward terms (i.e. twice)
prescription drug label instructions.
We would like to thank Mary Bocchini, Katherine Davis,
Prescription drug labels often are the only print source of
and Silvia Skripkauskas for their assistance in implementing
dosage instructions received by patients. Given the tangible
nature of the prescription bottle, these label instructions may be
Funding: Dr. Wolf is supported by a career development
the ‘last line’ of informational support detailing how and when
award through the Centers for Disease Control and Prevention
a patient should administer a prescribed medicine. Yet many of
the common phrases used to describe dosage instructions areinadequately written and contribute to misunderstanding.
Patients with limited literacy may face greater difficulty whenattempting to infer and interpret instructions. Research is
[1] Institute of Medicine. In: Aspden P, Wolcott J, Bootman L, Cronenwett
needed to evaluate the use of enhanced strategies of
LR, editors. Preventing medication errors. Washington, DC: National
communicating dosage information and warnings for improv-
ing comprehension among patients across all literacy levels. In
[2] Davis TC, Wolf MS, Bass PF, Tilson H, Neuberger M, Parker RM.
the end, all patients would benefit from more clearly presented
Literacy and misunderstanding of prescription drug labels. Ann InternMed 2006;145:887–94.
[3] Davis TC, Wolf MS, Bass PF, Middlebrooks M, Kennan E, Baker DW,
Bennett CL, Durazo-Arvizu R, Savory S, Parker RM. Low literacy impairs
comprehension of prescription drug warning labels. J Gen Intern Med2006;21:847–51.
Unfortunately, there are only minimal standards and
[4] Wolf MS, Davis TC, Bass PF, Tilson H, Parker RM. Misunderstanding
prescription drug warning labels among patients with low literacy. Am J
regulations set by state boards of pharmacy that dictate any
Health System Pharm 2006;63:1048–55.
recommendations for content and organization of prescription
[5] Shrank W, Avorn J, Rolon C, Shekelle P. Effect of content and format of
drug labels . As such, rules vary by state. Our research
prescription drug labels on readability, understanding, and medication use:
study provides initial guidance for improving the dosage
a systematic review. Ann Pharmacother 2007;41:783–801.
instructions on prescription bottles, and ‘best practices’ can
[6] Dunn EB, Wolfe JJ. Let go of Latin! Vet Hum Toxicol 2001;43:235–6. [7] Gupta AK, Cooper EA, Feldman SR, Fleischer Jr AB, Balkrishnan R.
be derived from our study. These are supported by health
Analysis of factors associated with increased prescription illegibility:
literacy principles and cognitive/human factors research
results from the National Ambulatory Medical Care Survey. Am J Manag
M.S. Wolf et al. / Patient Education and Counseling 67 (2007) 293–300
[8] Institute of Medicine. In: Nielsen-Bohlman L, Panzer A, Kindig DA,
[22] Davis TC, Michielutte R, Askov EN, Williams MV, Weiss BD. Practical
editors. Health literacy: a prescription to end confusion. Washington, DC:
assessment of adult literacy in healthcare. Health Ed Beh 1998;25:613–24.
[23] Davis TC, Kennen EM, Gazmararian JA, Williams MV. Literacy testing in
[9] Institute of Medicine. In: Kohn L, Corrigan J, Donaldson M, editors. To
In: Schwartzberg JG, VanGeest JB, Wang CC,
err is human: building a safer health system. Washington, DC: National
editors. Understanding health literacy: implications for medicine and
public health. Chicago, IL: AMA Press; 2004.
[10] Tarn DM, Heritage J, Paterniti DA, Hays RD, Kravitz RL, Wenger NS.
[24] Parker RM, Baker DW, Williams MV, Nurss JR. The test of functional
Physician communication when prescribing new medications. Arch Intern
health literacy in adults: a new instrument for measuring patients’ literacy
skills. J Gen Intern Med 1995;10:537–41.
[11] Morris LA, Tabak ER, Gondel K. Counseling patients about prescribed
[25] Strauss A, Corbin J. Basics of qualitative research: grounded theory
medications: 12-year trend. Med Care 1997;35:996–1007.
and techniques. Newbury Park, CA: Sage Publications; 1990
[12] Metlay JP, Cohen A, Polsky D, Kimmel SE, Koppel R, Hennessy S.
[26] Miles MB, Huberman AM. Qualitative data analysis: an expanded
Medication safety in older adults: home-based practice patterns. J Am
sourcebook, 2nd ed., Thousand Oaks, CA: Sage Publications; 1994.
[27] Emerson RM, Fretz RI, Shaw LL. Writing ethnographic fieldnotes.
[13] Wolf MS, Davis TC, Shrank W, Neuberger M, Parker RM. A critical
Chicago: University of Chicago Press; 1995.
review of FDA-approved medication guides. Pat Educ Counsel
[28] Medical Expenditure Panel Survey [on-line]. Available at
[14] Krass I, Svarstad BL, Bultman D. Using alternative methodologies for
[29] Morrell RW, Park DC, Poon LW. Quality of instructions on prescription
evaluating patient medication leaflets. Pat Educ Counsel 2002;47:29–
drug labels: effects on memory and comprehension in young and old
adults. Gerontologist 1989;29:345–54.
[15] Gustafsson J, Kalvemark S, Nilsson G, Nilsson JL. Patient information
[30] Park DC, Morrell RW, Frieske D, Blackburn AB, Birchmore D. Cognitive
leaflets: patients’ comprehension of information about interactions and
factors and the use of over-the-counter medication organizers by arthritis
contraindications. Pharm World Sci 2005;27:35–40.
patients. Hum Factors 1991;33:57–67.
[16] van den Broek P, Kremer K. The mind in action: what it means to
[31] Doak CC, Doak LG, Root JH. Teaching patients with low-literacy skills,
comprehend. In: Taylor B, van den Broek P, Graves M, editors. Reading
2nd ed., Philadelphia, PA: JB Lippincott; 1996.
for meaning. New York: Teacher’s College Press; 2000.
[32] Root J, Stableford S. Write it easy-to-read: a guide to creating plan English
[17] Rapp DN, van den Broek P. Dynamic text comprehension: an integrative
materials (especially for the Medicaid market). Biddeford, Maine: Health
view of reading. Cur Direct Psychol Sci 2005;14:276–9.
Literacy Center, University of New England; 1998.
[18] Rapp DN, Samuel A. A reason for rhyme: phonological and semantic
[33] Park DC, Morrell RW, Shifren K, editors. Processing of medical informa-
influences on lexical access. J Exp Psychol Learn 2002;28:564–71.
tion in aging patients: cognitive and human factors perspectives. Mahwah,
[19] American Association for Public Opinion Research. Standard definitions:
NJ: Lawrence Erlbaum Associates; 1999.
final dispositions of case codes and outcome rates for surveys, 3rd ed.,
[34] Bull SA, Hu XH, Hunkeler EM, Lee JY, Ming EE, Markson LE, Fireman
B. Discontinuation of use and switching of antidepressants: influence of
[20] Willis SL, Jay GM, Diehl M, Marsiske M. Longitudinal change and
patient–physician communication. JAMA 2002;288:1403–9.
prediction of everyday task competence in the elderly. Res Aging
[35] Swanson JM, Forrest K, Ledbetter C, Hall S, Holstine EJ, Shafer MR.
Readability of commercial and generic contraceptive instructions. Image J
[21] Willis SL, Dolan MM, Bertrand RM. Problem solving on health-related
tasks of daily living. In: Park DC, Morrell RW, Shiffren D, editors.
[36] Ngoh LN, Shepherd MD. Design, development, and evaluation of visual
Processing of medical information in aging patients. Mahwah, NJ:
aids for communicating prescription drug instructions to nonliterate
Lawrence Ehrlbaum Associates; 1999. p. 199–222.
patients in rural Cameroon. Pat Educ Counsel 1997;30(3):257–70.
OzmepTM Enteric-coated Tablets contain the active ingredient omeprazole CONSUMER Reflux Oesophagitis MEDICINE by the stomach producing too much acid. Depending INFORMATION What is in this duodenal ulcer. A gastric ulcer occurs in the stomach Peptic Ulcers associated with Helicobacter pylori infection It does not take the place of talking to your doctor or