In H. Friedman, T.W. Klein & A.L. Friedman (Eds.), Psychoneuroimmunology, stress and infection, CRC Press, Boca Raton, 1995 (pp. 1-21
Historical Perspectives on Psychoneuroimmunology Robert Ader Center for Psychoneuroimmunology Research University of Rochester School of Medicine and Dentistry
Running head: HISTORICAL PERSPECTIVES Correspondence:
Center for Psychoneuroimmunology Research
most simply, to the study of the interactions
among behavioral, neural and endocrine (or neuroendocrine), and immunological
processes of adaptation. Its central premise is that homeostasis is an integrated
process involving interactions among the nervous, endocrine and immune
“systems.” The term was first used in 1980, in my presidential address to the
American Psychosomatic Society.1 Its most conspicuous use was as the title of an
edited volume2 which one reviewer referred to prophetically as “The signature
volume of a new field of research.” This first volume was an attempt to bring
together emerging research suggesting a relationship between the brain and the
immune system. Traditionally, however, the immune system has been considered
an autonomous agency of defense — a system of bodily defenses regulated by
cellular interactions that are independent of neural influences. Besides, there were
no known connections between the brain and the immune system. To be sure, it
was known that hormones or, at least, adrenal hormones could influence
immunity; some investigators were aware that brain lesions could influence
immune responses; and it was also known or, at least, suspected that emotional
states were associated with the development or progression of diseases related to
the immune system. Few scientists at that time, however, took such observations
too seriously. After all, there were no mechanistic explanations for how such
Considering the brief time during which multidisciplinary research has
addressed brain-immune system interactions, a great deal of data has accumulated
in support of the proposition that homeostatic mechanisms are the product of an
integrated system of defenses of which the immune system is one component.3
Autonomic nervous system activity and neuroendocrine outflow via the pituitary
can influence immune function, and cytokines and hormones released by an
activated immune system can influence neural and endocrine processes.
Regulatory peptides and receptors once confined to the brain are expressed by
both the nervous and immune systems and each system is thereby capable of
modulating the activities of the other. It is hardly surprising, then, to find that
immunologic reactivity can be modified by Pavlovian conditioning — or that the
behavioral and emotional states that accompany the perception of and the effort to
adapt to events in the real world can influence immune responses. Thus,
psychoneuroimmunology successfully challenged the commonly held assumption
of an autonomous immune system. One may, therefore, entertain the proposition
that changes in immune function mediate the effects of psychosocial factors and
stressful life experiences on the susceptibility to and/or the precipitation or
It is not my intent, in this introductory chapter, to review the literature
outlining the history of psychoneuroimmunology. I have, instead, taken my
charge literally and chosen the more manageable task of presenting here some
editorial comments and some historical perspectives on psychoneuroimmunology.
These are, of necessity, brief and selected and only cover developments up until
about 1980. Very much more could be written about the people and the findings
described here because these are rich personal stories. Much more could also be
written about what was contemporary and what came before and what came after
the 1970s, but this is a chapter — not a book. The research I have chosen to
highlight was not necessarily even the first of its kind; in my opinion, however,
the systematic research initiated during the 1970s was “the right stuff at the right
time.” No one study can be said to have been (or could have been) responsible for
psychoneuroimmunology. I suspect that none of the research initiatives described
below would have had quite the same impact had it not been for the converging
evidence of brain-immune system interactions being provided by the others at
about the same time. Studies of brain-immune system relationships had been
appearing in the literature for many, many years. However, it was the coalescence
of research initiated during the 1970s and sustained thereafter — and the identity
provided by the label, psychoneuroimmunology, itself — that reawakened long-
standing interests and attracted new investigators into this “new” field.
The notion of integration is neither new nor, for the most part, can it be
considered controversial. It was David Hamburg, I think, who pointed out that
biochemistry, a hybrid discipline, was initially viewed as a combination of poor
biology and weak chemistry. Today, it is basic and central to the study of
medicine. Psychopharmacology is a recognition of the fact that drug effects
depend to a large extent on the state of the organism into whom they are
introduced. Neuroendocrinology reflects an appreciation of the fact that the
functions of the endocrine system can not be fully understood without reference to
its interactions with the nervous system. And psychoneuroendocrinology
acknowledges that the feedback and feedforward pathways between these
“systems” influence and are influenced by behavior. Hybrid disciplines are not
always or solely attempts at integration or synthesis. Basic fields such as
neurochemistry or immunopharmacology, and clinical subspecialties such as
neuropsychiatry, for example, designate a focus within a parent “discipline.” In
fact, in keeping with the zeitgeist of the biomedical model, the latter reductionistic
referent is probably the more common one.
Among other shortcomings, disciplinary boundaries tend to keep insiders in
and outsiders out. Hybrid disciplines have nevertheless emerged and significantly
extended our understanding of the functions of the components of interacting
systems. Why is it, then, that psychoneuroimmunology precipitated — and, in
some circles, continues to engender — so much resistance and enmity? Certainly,
the attention that psychoneuroimmunology has captured in the popular press and
its exploitation by those who redefine and use psychoneuroimmunology as the
scientific umbrella for their own undisciplined and untested theories and practices
cannot have endeared psychoneuroimmunology or investigators who study brain-
immune system interactions to the remainder of the scientific community. In my
unsubstantiated view, however, the reasons lie as much within as without the
biomedical community. Some scientists are willing to say they “don’t believe”
there’s anything of substance in psychoneuroimmunology, although they are not
necessarily willing to be quoted. Of course, scientists do not have recourse to “I
don’t believe it” as grounds for rejecting hypotheses. One can argue, “I don’t
believe it because.” as in: “I don’t believe it because there are no connections
between the brain and the immune system.” Such arguments are capable of
disproof and, with respect to psychoneuroimmunology, all such arguments have
been contradicted by experimental data. Unfortunately for the development of the
field, however, there are those in influential positions who, purportedly, believed
that psychoneuroimmunology would go nowhere and acted in a consultative
capacity on this “belief.” There is, too, a sense of unease among some so-called
“hard” scientists who seem to view the scientific study of behavior as an
oxymoron. In truth, the sophistication in experimental design and analysis of
research by the behavioral sciences far exceeds that of the more classical
biomedical sciences and even molecular biology, and is essential for addressing
the quantitative questions (e.g., when, how much, under what conditions) that are
raised by factoring behavioral, neural and endocrine variables into the
experimental analysis of immunoregulatory processes.
Within the field, there have been some minor battles over “turf,’ but none has
altered the defining theme of the field. The emergence of
psychoneuroimmunology has actually broadened some fields of study that were
more narrowly defined in the recent past (e.g., papers in psychoneuroimmunology
are now solicited for publication in the Journal of Neuroimmunology).
“Neuroimmunomodulation” and “neuroendocrinimmunology,” mere
mispronunciations of psychoneuroimmunology, seem to have been precipitated to
disengage from the study of behavior and/or to more specifically brand the field
with one’s own personal or disciplinary irons. Neither label changed the
substance of the interdisciplinary research it promoted. (Of course, if you can
come up with still another name, you, too, can also come up with another “First
The first sustained program of research were the studies of Russian
investigators on the classical conditioning of immune responses. This research,
derived from a Pavlovian perspective, began with Metal’nikoff and Chorine4 who
were working at the Pasteur Institute in Paris. This research was reviewed in
English in 1933 by no less than Clark Hull,5 a renowned learning theorist of that
era. It was also reviewed in 1933 and, again, in 1941 by Kopeloff.6,7 The only
other substantive review of this literature in English appeared in
Psychoneuroimmunology.8 None of these early reviews attracted much attention
or had any impact on research outside the then Soviet Union, including the studies
of brain lesions on immune reactions and the physiologic studies of stress derived
from the work of Hans Selye. Even the research implicating the nervous system in
the modulation of immune responses initiated by Rasmussen and his colleagues
and others in the 1950s and 60s failed to attract much sustained attention from any
Aaron Frederick Rasmussen, Jr. was certainly one of the earliest pioneers of
psychoneuroimmunology. His association with Norman Brill, then Chair of the
Department of Psychiatry at the UCLA School of Medicine, was probably the first
collaboration between a microbiologist/immunologist and a behavioral scientist.
Rasmussen died in 1984, at the age of 68, after serving as Chair of the Department
of Medical Microbiology and Immunology (1962-1969) and thereafter as
Associate Dean of the School of Medicine. He is remembered as a beloved and
inspiring teacher and colleague and an outstanding virologist whose genetic
studies laid the foundation for understanding the notorious worldwide variability
in influenza viruses. Rasmussen was a meticulous experimental microbiologist,
who, at the same time, never lost site of host factors in disease. He was intrigued
by the unproved conventional wisdom that emotional states influence the course of
infectious illness, as depicted by such great novelists as Thomas Mann and as
observed by such great pre-modern clinicians as Sir William Osler. An integrative
thinker not bound by disciplinary lines, Rasmussen sought out Brill to discuss his
In 1957, Rasmussen, Marsh and Brill demonstrated that a stressful experience,
avoidance conditioning, could increase the susceptibility of mice to herpes
simplex virus. In a series of landmark papers, the pathogenic effect of emotional
stress on animals exposed to herpes virus,9 Coxsackie B virus,10 and vesicular
stomatitis virus11 was explored. He and his coworkers also found decreased
susceptibility to poliomyelitis virus in stressed monkeys, an early demonstration of
the variability in stress effects on disease susceptibility.12 Anticipating modern
work in psycho-oncology as well as psychoneuroimmunology, Rasmussen and his
colleagues also found that stress influenced the malignancy of polyoma virus in
mice,13 and his later work on stress included measures of viral antibodies and
interferon production.14,15 I regret very much that I never met Fred Rasmussen.
His research set the stage for a variety of studies dealing with stress and infection,
such as those initiated by Friedman, Glasgow and Ader,16,17 and Solomon’s
studies on stress and antibody responses to a novel bacterial antigen.18 (It may
not be scientifically noteworthy, but it is of personal interest that my colleague,
Nicholas Cohen, was a postdoctoral student in Rasmussen’s department at UCLA
in the mid 1960s. Unfortunately, Rasmussen’s involvement in this research was
decreasing which may explain why it took Cohen so long to enter the field.)
George Solomon was another of the early investigators to show that
psychological or environmental stressors could influence immunity. He and his
colleague, Rudolf Moos, made painstaking observations of the life histories and
personality characteristics of patients, seeking a clue to the frequently observed
association between emotional states and the onset or exacerbation of arthritis.
Solomon described the area as “psychoimmunology” and, despite the concerns of
some of his colleagues, hung a sign to that effect on his laboratory door. In
retrospect, Solomon’s perspective on psychoneuroimmunology derived from
curiosity, serendipity, psychodynamics, and the organization of disparate
observations. Equally important, he claims, is the role of tenacity, frustration
tolerance, and the ability to accept the encouragement of some and to reject the
negativism of others in the development of new observations and theories.
Initially, Solomon was interested in psychological factors in the onset and
course of autoimmune disease.19 This interest was instigated by his father, a
psychiatrist, who was convinced that psychological factors played a role in the
onset and course of rheumatoid arthritis. As a resident in psychiatry at the
Langley Porter Institute, he and W. Jeffrey Fessel studied patients with systemic
lupus erythematosus (SLE) who had severe psychiatric symptoms. The similarity
of the symptoms in SLE to the symptoms seen in schizophrenia, prompted
Solomon to ask whether schizophrenia could be an autoimmune disease of the
brain with genetic and psychological predisposing factors that could be influenced
by stressful life experiences. After a stint in the army, Solomon returned to the
University of California in San Francisco and to research on immunoglobulins and
He also joined forces and established a productive collaboration with Rudolf
Moos, a psychologist, who was studying psychosocial factors in rheumatoid
arthritis (RA). Solomon and Moos later joined the Department of Psychiatry at
Stanford, but continued to do their research at UCSF; at Stanford, they
encountered difficulties in obtaining access to arthritic patients for such
“psychological nonsense.” The most unusual study in their series of papers on
rheumatoid arthritis21 was the one comparing physically healthy relatives of RA
patients (known to have a greater than average likelihood of developing
autoimmune disease) with the RA patients themselves — with the additional
consideration of whether or not their sera contained rheumatoid factor, an anti IgG
antibody characteristic of rheumatoid arthritis. Neither subjects nor examiners
knew the sera status of the study population. Those who were negative for the
rheumatoid factor were like a general population: normally distributed from
psychologically healthy to psychologically disturbed. However, rheumatoid factor
positive relatives of RA patients were psychologically “healthy,” lacking anxiety,
depression, or alienation and reporting good relationships with spouses, friends,
and relatives. Psychological well-being seemed to exert a protective influence in
the face of a probable genetic predisposition to autoimmune disease.
The future, Solomon thought, lay in mechanistic studies — and, what’s more,
he saw an opening. In 1963, he read a paper by Robert Good22 who postulated a
relationship between autoimmunity and relative immunologic incompetence.
Frank Dixon23 related such incompetence to the pathogenic formation of antigen-
antibody complexes which occur when the amount of antibody is low in relation
to antigen. Solomon immediately strung together: immunologic incompetence,
adrenocorticosteroid hormones and immunosuppression with stress and
corticosteroids. A naive and simplistic notion, he thought, but a heuristic one,
nonetheless. These notions were presented in “Emotions, immunity, and disease:
A speculative theoretical integration” published in 1964.24 Solomon attempted to
conscript Moos into developing a laboratory in which they could stress rodents.
“After all, no one,” he thought, “was going to believe clinical data, but they will
be convinced by animal experiments.” Moos, however, was not an
experimentalist and chose to pursue other interests, so Solomon was on his own.
He was provided with a laboratory, but he recognized that he knew virtually no
immunology. Practically all the immunologists with whom he spoke told him that
the immune system was autonomous, totally self-regulatory, and, thus, not subject
to neuroendocrine influences. Nevertheless, Solomon established his
“psychoimmunology laboratory.” Although he had the support and tutelage of
good people, he was unable to develop the necessary assay procedures for this
work and considered giving up this line of research. Instead, he contacted one of
the most noted immunologists in the world, Sir MacFarlane Burnet, who had
revolutionized immunology with his clonal-selection theory of antibody
formation. In response to Solomon’s letter, Sir MacFarlane Burnet replied: “I am
most skeptical, but your ideas are interesting. Why don’t you come to Melbourne?
We’ll talk, and my successor, Gus (now Sir Gustav) Nossal will teach you simple
techniques for stress studies.” Solomon did go to Melbourne where he claims he
learned something about immunology and a great deal about immunologists.
In the ensuing years, Solomon was able to enlist the collaboration of gifted
colleagues with whom he conducted some of the first studies that now fall under
the rubric of “psychoneuroimmunology.” With Thomas Merigan, who ran the
tedious bioassay, he studied the effects of stress and steroids on interferon
production.25 Using flagellin, a bacterial antigen, it was shown that handling
during early life could influence subsequent primary and secondary antibody
responses in rats,26 and that different stressors have different affects on antibody
production.18 He also developed a collaboration with an immunologist, Alfred
Amkraut, whom Solomon describes as “brave, most competent, obsessively
meticulous, and cautious.” Solomon and Amkraut studied the effects of stress on
virus-induced tumors,27 graft-vs-host reactions,28 adjuvant-induced arthritis,29
and other immunologic reactions. “Nobody, however, was listening.” Among
other things, “Alfred was not given tenure (‘What does the CNS have to do with
immunology?’).” Thus, in the early 1970’s, Solomon closed the door on this line
of research. Ten years later, however, he was to return.
Solomon kept close watch on the developments in psychoneuroimmunology,
“.especially after the publication of Ader and Cohen’s conditioning work.”
Having been asked to contribute to the first edition of Psychoneuroimmunology,
he concluded that “PNI was on the map at last,” and returned to the field. AIDS
was suspected of being infectious, it involved immune abnormality, and it could
also affect the CNS. AIDS, then, seemed the ideal condition to study within a
psychoneuroimmunologic frame of reference. In 1983, Solomon moved from
Fresno to the home campus of the University of California in San Francisco to join
the incipient biopsychosocial AIDS project designed to seek psychologic-
immunologic (AIDS progression) correlations. There he pursued his long-
standing interest in “exceptions to the rule,” namely long-term survivors with
AIDS from whom he felt one might learn what psychological factors and
mediating mechanisms contributed to health and longevity. Their informally
studied group of long-term survivors were remarkable people.30 One of these was
singer Michael Callen who wrote about the study in his book, Surviving AIDS.
Callen, who died in 1994 after 12 years of symptomatic AIDS, personified what
Solomon was attempting to explain. Another personal encounter that colored
Solomon’s perspective on psychoneuroimmunology was his association with
Norman Cousins. It was Cousins’ interest in understanding the role of attitude in
healing that led L.J. (“Jolly”) West, then Chair of the Department of Psychiatry
and Biobehavioral Sciences, to invite Solomon to join the faculty at the University
of California in Los Angeles. Cousins founded a UCLA Task Force on
Psychoneuroimmunology of which Solomon is still a member. In addition to
continuing work on AIDS, Solomon is currently engaged in some psychologically
“upper” research: research on “very healthy old people instead of sick young
One of the channels of communication between the neuroendocrine and
immune systems is achieved through the receptors that exist on immune cells.
John Hadden was prompted to ask if lymphocytes had adrenergic receptors by the
emergence of adenylate cyclase as the beta receptor transduction unit in many
tissues and, most specifically, by “The beta adrenergic theory of the atopic
abnormality in bronchial asthma” proposed by Ando Szentivanyi.31 Based on
studies in guinea pigs of the effects of hypothalamic lesions and stimulation on
anaphylactic responses,32,33 the first of such studies on brain lesions and
immune reaction, Szentivanyi suggested that the CNS had an impact on the
immune system, at least in terms of allergic mechanisms. He further postulated a
blockade of beta adrenergic receptors, with a resulting exaggeration of immune
responses, as a cause of asthma. That is, it was hypothesized that beta adrenergic
receptors acting via the adenylate cyclase/cyclic AMP system would down
It was during his first year of a medical fellowship with Elliot Middleton, Jr.
that Hadden learned of Szentivanyi’s formulation and set out to determine if
lymphocytes had adrenergic receptors that could regulate immune function in a
meaningful way. Hadden and his associates showed that, in the presence of
hydrocortisone, alpha-adrenergic stimulation augmented and beta adrenergic
stimulation inhibited the lymphoproliferative response to the mitogen, PHA.34
This was the first observation linking lymphocytes to the sympathetic nervous
system, opening a wide door to the study of neural influences on immunity.
These findings led Hadden in several directions. The notion that beta
antagonists and cyclic AMP down-regulated lymphocyte proliferation was
pursued by several investigators and confirmed for a variety of lymphocyte
functions.35 Hadden and his associates elaborated on the newly detected alpha
adrenergic effects as these related to glucose metabolism and transmembrane K+
transport, finally linking them to direct effects on membrane ATPases of
lymphocytes.36-38 While working in Minneapolis on transmembrane signaling,
Hadden was introduced to cyclic GMP by Nelson Goldberg. Together, they found
that cyclic GMP was involved in the signal induced in lymphocytes by PHA.39
They also found that cyclic GMP was involved in lymphocyte cholinergic
responses. While in the process of developing these observations, Terry Strom
presented the first paper to show that T lymphocyte cytotoxicity was augmented
by muscarinic cholinergic stimulation.40 Hadden and his associates extended
these results, demonstrating stimulation of RNA and DNA synthesis of
lymphocytes and implicating cyclic GMP in the process.41,42 These observations
were the first to link lymphocytes to the parasympathetic nervous system, opening
a door to immune regulation by the entire autonomic nervous system.
Hadden initiated some additional in vivo studies, but became allergic to the
animals and had to abandon this line of research. Besides, he was then
preoccupied with questions about signal transduction mechanisms. He was not
unmindful and, indeed, was fascinated by Robert Good’s stories about his personal
involvement in successful demonstrations of hypnotically-induced alterations of
immunity,43 but it appeared to him that the study of the neural modulation of
immunity was not yet ready to surface as a bona fide area of research. Yet, it was
in 1980 that Hadden, instrumental in the organization of the new International
Conferences on Immunopharmacology and starting a new journal, the
International Journal of Immunopharmacology, invited me to present at this
immunology meeting. It was important, he thought, that the
immunopharmacologist be made aware of the research and the implications of the
work on conditioning and immunity. “Now, 25 years later,” Hadden writes, “I
recognize that it has emerged and I am happy to have contributed some impetus.”
As evidenced by recent work on the endocrinology of the thymus,44,45 Hadden
remains committed to an understanding of neuroendocrine-immune
When asked how I became involved in psychoneuroimmunology, I can not
refer to a logical starting point. I say it was an accident; I was “forced” into it by
my data. I was studying taste aversion learning in rats. When a novel,
distinctively-flavored conditioned stimulus (CS), saccharin, is paired with the
unconditioned effects of a drug, cyclophosphamide (CY), which induces a
transient stomach upset, the animal learns in one such conditioning trial to avoid
saccharin-favored drinking solutions. We were conducting an experiment on the
acquisition and extinction of the conditioned aversive response as a function of the
strength of the CS, i.e., the volume of saccharin consumed before the animal was
injected with CY. As expected, the magnitude of the conditioned response was
directly related to the volume of saccharin consumed on the single conditioning
trial. Also, repeated presentations of the CS in the absence of the drug resulted in
extinction of the aversive response, and the rate of extinction was inversely related
to the magnitude of the CS. However, in the course of these extinction trials,
animals began to die. A troublesome but uninteresting observation. As more
animals died, it became evident that mortality, like the magnitude of the
conditioned response, varied directly with the volume of saccharin consumed on
the one drug trial; a troublesome but interesting effect.
As a psychologist, I was unaware that there were no connections between the
brain and the immune system. Therefore, I was free to make up any story I
wanted in an attempt to explain this orderly relationship. The hypothesis was that,
in the course of conditioning the avoidance behavior, we were also conditioning
the immunosuppressive effects of cyclophosphamide. If, every time the
conditioned animals were reexposed to the CS previously paired with the drug, the
CS induced a conditioned immunosuppressive response, these animals might be
more susceptible to low levels of pathogenic stimulation that may have existed in
the laboratory environment. Moreover, if the strength of the conditioned response
was a function of the magnitude of the CS, the greater the immunosuppressive
response, the greater the likelihood of an increased susceptibility to environmental
pathogens. Thus, it was the serendipitous observation of mortality in a simple
conditioning study and the need to explain an orderly relationship between
mortality and a conditioned aversive response that gave rise to the hypothesis that
immune responses could be modified by conditioning operations.
A Letter to the Editor describing these observations and the speculation that
immune responses were subject to conditioning was published in Psychosomatic Medicine in 1974.46 It was a draft of this letter that elicited the first of many
unexpected and sometimes frightening responses to this work. George Engel
who, having criticized me for being too conservative in the past, said that, based
on my conservative reputation, people were going to believe this, just because I
said it. Although meant as a compliment, I found the prospect somewhat
frightening; I had not given up my right to be wrong. I was to learn, however, that
if you say something unimportant, it doesn’t matter whether you’re right or wrong;
if, however, you say something that could be important, you had better be right!
People listened politely, but I did not have much luck in generating any interest
in this hypothesis — let alone the help I would need to examine it — until I met
Nicholas Cohen. Cohen was the first person with sophistication in immunology
who didn’t think these notions were too “far out.” Thus began a collaboration
that is as active today as it was in 1974. Still oblivious of the Russian studies of
the 1920s, Cohen and I designed a study to directly examine the hypothesis that
immune responses could be modified by classical conditioning. For better or
worse (sometimes, we’re not sure), the first experimental paradigm we adopted
was successful and, with some evident trepidation on the part of the reviewers and
editor, “Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression” was published in 1975.47
This study demonstrated that, like other physiological processes, the immune
system was subject to classical (Pavlovian) conditioning, providing dramatic
evidence of an inextricable relationship between the brain and the immune system.
Essentially, we were forced to the conclusion that there was a relationship between
the brain and the immune system. The biomedical community, however, was, to
be generous, guarded — and, to be precise, quite negative. Such a phenomenon
simply could not occur because, as everybody knew, there were no connections
between the brain and the immune system. Seminar groups were assigned the
(unsuccessful) task of finding out what we had done wrong. The first replication
of our findings48 came from a study originally intended to show that, taking
appropriate care and using more accurate assay procedures, the effect would not
occur. The National Institutes of Health and National Institute of Mental Health,
however, were not “forced” to the conclusion that one could condition alterations
in immunologic reactivity, and, notwithstanding George Engel’s predictions,
Study Sections were loathe to use my conservative reputation as collateral.
However, reading between the lines of “pink sheets” (and as confirmed by Study
Section members much later), we might be right — and could they take that
chance for so little money — and for only two years at a time? Our initial study
was supported by a one-year grant from the Grant Foundation (where my
reputation was collateral) and then, reluctantly, it seems, we were funded by the
NIH. At that time, ours was the only NIH grant in this area which, on renewal,
was thereafter supported by the NIMH. Today, a computer search of
“psychoneuroimmunology” and “neuroimmunomodulation” lists more than 200
active research grants being supported by the U.S. Public Health Service.*
Over the next several years, there were replications and major extensions of
conditioned alterations of humoral and cell-mediated immune responses.49-51
Recent work has successfully used antigen, itself, as the unconditioned stimulus.
A classically conditioned enhancement of antibody production occurred when
conditioned mice were reexposed to the conditioned stimulus in the context of
reexposure to a minimally immunogenic dose of that same antigen.52 These and
earlier experiments53 documented the conditioning of immune responses, per se,
in contrast to the conditioning of immunopharmacologic responses. Studies in
New Zealand mice genetically susceptible to a systemic lupus erythematosus-like
disease were used to demonstrate the biologic impact of conditioned alterations in
immune responses. Substituting CSs for active drug on some scheduled treatment
days delayed the onset of autoimmune disease using a cumulative amount of
immunosuppressive drug that was ineffective by itself in altering the progression
of disease.54 Similarly, reexposure to a CS previously paired with
immunosuppressive drug treatment prolonged the survival of foreign tissue grafted
onto mice.55,56 Such results have yet to be verified in human patients. However,
there has been one clinical case study describing the successful use of
conditioning in reducing by one half the amount of cytoxan therapy received by a
To date, the neural, endocrine, or neuroendocrine mechanisms underlying
conditioned alterations in immune function are unknown — reason enough,
apparently, for some biomedical scientists to reject the phenomenon, itself, or, in
the case of Nature, to reject for publication a paper demonstrating conditioned
enhancement of antibody production without even providing a review. One can
only wonder about the implications of applying uniformly the criterion of having
to identify “.the precise mechanisms involved in the phenomenon you observe.”
in order to publish experimental results. Besides the fact that the precise
mechanisms underlying behaviorally-induced changes in immune function are not
known, it is also true that in only a few instances has the functional significance of
the bidirectional communication pathways that have been identified among the
nervous, endocrine and immune systems been determined.
To be sure, our studies were not always maligned. I recall, for example, the
evening I met Lewis Thomas whom I have always thought of as the Montaigne of
the biological sciences. After a brief exchange of pleasantries, Thomas said, “You
sure are making life difficult for some people.”
“Well,” I answered, slowly — trying to think of an appropriate response, “as I
read Lewis Thomas, that shouldn’t bother you.”
“It doesn’t,” he replied, “I love it!”
During these same years, Hugo Besedovsky was beginning his studies on
endocrine-immune system interactions. Besedovsky was led into
psychoneuroimmunology through a clinical route. Trained as a pediatrician at the
Medical Faculty of Rosario in Argentina, he was confronted daily with patients
with infectious and other diseases involving the immune system. Having been
“hybridized” early in his training, Besedovsky naturally viewed the immune
system as operating within the context of other physiological processes.
Reflecting his pediatric training, his first studies in the early 1970s addressed
endocrine influences on the immune and haematopoietic systems during
ontogeny.58 He focused on adrenocortical function which, at the time, was the
only endocrine activity known to affect immunity. He discussed his interest in the
possibility that neuroendocrine mechanisms could contribute to immunoregulation
with Professor Bernardo Houssay who encouraged him to work with Sir Peter
Medawar in London. Medawar accepted but then could not accommodate
Besedovsky in his laboratory because of illness, so he went to the Swiss Research
Institute in Davos, Switzerland where he was fortunate to have Ernst Sorkin as a
Besedovsky’s research on the neuroendocrine regulation of immune responses
was, and still is, based on the premise that immune responses are a part of
integrated homeostatic mechanisms under the control of the nervous and
endocrine systems. Thus, he reasoned, it should be possible to provide evidence
that: (1) antigen exposure initiates a flow of information to neuroendocrine
structures about changes in the activity of immune cells; (2) as a consequence of
this information, an efferent neuroendocrine response should be elicited; and (3)
this efferent response should have functional significance for immunoregulatory
Besedovsky and his colleagues proceeded to demonstrate in two animal
species that, independent of any “stress-induced” responses related to the
procedures, immunization with different antigens was capable of inducing
endocrine changes (an increase in corticosterone and a decrease in thyroxin) that
were under CNS control.59 This was followed by a collaboration with Professor
Dominik Felix from the Brain Research Institute in Zurich which established that
there was, in parallel with the production of antibody, an increase in the firing rate
of neurons within the ventromedial hypothalamus.60,61 This was a dramatic
demonstration that the nervous system is capable of responding to signals emitted
by an immune response. These results, Besedovsky recalls, were first submitted to
Nature which rejected the paper “because it is self evident that the brain must
receive information from the immune system.”
Hugo Besedovsky’s professional and personal relationship with Adriana del
Rey, also from Argentina, began when she joined the Institute in Davos in 1977.
Their first collaborative research concerned antigenic competition and the
immunosuppressive role of elevations in adrenocortical steroids.62 These studies
supported their hypothesis that glucocorticoid elevations associated with antigen
exposure act to prevent an abnormal expansion of the immune response which
might otherwise result in a cumulative, excessive immune cell proliferation
favoring the expression of autoimmune and lymphoproliferative processes and the
production of potentially harmful products of activated lymphocytes.
Analogous experiments on the involvement of the sympathetic nervous system
in immunoregulation included the measurement of the content and the turnover
rate of splenic noradrenaline during an immune response. In highly reactive
animals, there is a decrease in noradrenaline content which occurs before the peak
in antibody titers;63,64 animals that have a less active immune system show an
increase in noradrenaline in lymphoid organs.65 Also, corresponding to the
increased activity of hypothalamic neurons during an immune response,
Besedovsky and his associates66 showed that there was a reduction in the
noradrenaline turnover rate in the hypothalamus and brain stem. Clearly, there
was a very dynamic interaction between the immune system and the sympathetic
nervous system that influenced immunoregulatory processes.
The fact that there were endocrine, autonomic and neural activity changes
during the course of immune responses indicated that the immune system could
convey information to the CNS which led Besedovsky to suggest that the immune
system acts as a “receptor sensorial organ.”60,66 This implies that the CNS can
sense the activity of the peripheral immune system involved in the recognition of
non-self intruders and modified self-components, as well. If so, the products of
immune cells should be able to affect neuroendocrine function. His approach
involved stimulating immune cells in vitro and transferring the supernatants
obtained from such cultures into naive animals. The culture supernatants induced
a pituitary-dependent increase in plasma corticosterone and a decrease in the
content of noradrenaline in the brain of the rats.67,68 Thus, Besedovsky provided
the first evidence that products of activated immune cells could affect endocrine
responses that were under CNS control. When purified lymphokines and
monokines became available in the 1980s, the laboratory began to study the
capacity of these immune system mediators (e.g., interleukin-1) to influence
neuroendocrine functions.69,70 Current research focuses on the effects of
endogenously produced lymphokines and monokines.
Current research also includes a concern for the potential clinical relevance of
neuroendocrine-immune system interactions. For example, some of the endocrine
changes effected by the inoculation of tumor cells are mediated by cytokines
rather than being a direct result of the tumor, itself, or the ensuing disease.71
Also, the pituitary-adrenal response to lipopolysaccharide is cytokine mediated72
and IL-1 is a main factor in activation of the pituitary-adrenal axis during viral
The innovative research initiated by Hugo Besedovsky, Adriana del Rey and
their colleagues has had a major impact on the acceptance of an integrated
approach to research on homeostatic processes, in general, and on
psychoneuroimmunology, in particular. It has also had a major impact on the
conceptualizations and on the directions of research coming from several
laboratories in the United States and in Europe. That, however, took time.
Initially, the response to their work, like the response experienced by others in the
field, was disheartening. On one of their several trips from Davos to Basel,
Besedovsky and del Rey met with Niels Jerne, then Director of the Institute of
Immunology, to discuss their ideas about the role of hormones and
neurotransmitters in immunoregulation with a world famous immunologist whom
Jerne listened and said, “This is too complicated. We still do not know many
things about the immune system, and I think we should know, for example,
whether there is a T cell receptor. Maybe you should work in vitro.” Needless to
say, this was an unexpected and upsetting response. About five years later,
Besedovsky was giving a seminar at the Hoffman-LaRoche Laboratories where
the first person to arrive was Neils Jerne. Following Besedovsky’s talk on the
immunomodulating effects of glucocorticoids, Jerne stood up and said that “I have
always believed that there is a communication between the immune and endocrine
Adriana del Rey, sitting near Jerne, interrupted him and shouted: “This is not
true! Five years ago you told us.(and she repeated the story).”
Of course, Jerne laughed and said, “Well, what I meant to say was that I have
always believed it but, after seeing these results, I think it may be true!”
(Parenthetically, Nicholas Cohen, who had spent a sabbatical year at the Basel
Institute in 1975, was invited to review our work on conditioning for a 1981
Besedovsky is now Professor of Physiology in the Medical Faculty of the
University of Marburg in Germany where he has established a Department of
Immunophysiology. The multidisciplinary expertise of his research group is still
devoted to investigations of the complex immune-neuro-endocrine interactions
that characterize the physiology of the immune system.
Similar thinking was directing the research of Edwin Blalock when, in 1979,
lymphocytes were discovered to be a source of brain peptide neurotransmitters and
pituitary hormones.75 These observations were the unexpected culmination of
three years of research when, as an Assistant Professor of Microbiology at the
University of Texas Medical Branch in Galveston, Blalock started out to
determine if the cytokine, interferon (IFN), could function as a hormone. Indeed,
it appeared that, among its other endocrine activities, interferon preparations could
stimulate the adrenal to synthesize glucocorticoids.76 Since the sequences of IFN
were not known at the time and IFN was functioning like ACTH, the primary
regulator of the adrenal gland, Blalock and his first postdoctoral fellow, Eric
Smith, wondered whether the steroidogenic activity of the cytokine might be due
to the presence of a residue ACTH-like sequence within the IFN molecule.
Although this appeared to be so,75 further studies, including the cloning of IFN,
showed that this was not the case.77 But, this research led to an even more
remarkable finding: supernatant fluids from human lymphocytes cultured with
IFN contained ACTH and the endogenous opioid peptides, endorphins.77
_Blalock remembers quite vividly the exhilaration they felt on the day they first
observed immunofluorescent pictures of lymphocytes staining positively for the
production of these substances. Such observations were indeed surprising since, at
the time, these peptides were thought to be the exclusive property of the brain and
pituitary gland. For Blalock — and for many others in the developing field of
psychoneuroimmunology — this discovery suggested a molecular approach for
solving the mystery of how the mind could control the immune system, e.g., how
classical conditioning might modify immunity. Such a relationship could exist
because the body’s two principle recognition organs, the brain and the immune
system, speak the same chemical language. If true, this meant that the immune
system could, indeed, talk back to the brain and, perhaps, alter physiology and
behavior. Research accomplished in the last several years confirms the fact that
such relationships do, in fact, exist78,79 and, because of the molecular and
biochemical nature of these studies a large measure of respectability was given to
psychoneuroimmunology — but, not immediately.
As with most, if not all discoveries that challenge current dogma, Blalock’s
work was met with healthy as well as unhealthy skepticism and, like many
pioneers in the field of psychoneuroimmunology, the messengers suffered
personal and professional indignities. The NIH site visitors reviewing their first
research grant proposal in this area concluded that Blalock and his colleagues
were actually sane and that the work had merit, but the project was funded for
only two years. According to Blalock, the study section’s message was clear: you
must sequence the lymphocyte’s ACTH to make your point unequivocally. In
retrospect, this was considered an impossible request made by Study Section
members who, according to Blalock, had never, themselves, sequenced anything.
However, as “green” investigators, who were also referred to as “biochemical
yahoos,” Blalock and his associates did not know enough to be daunted. After a
year of research and encouraging results, the Study Section members were still
unimpressed and Blalock’s application for renewal of this research was
disapproved. When later reviewed by scientists knowledgeable in the area, this
very same proposal was judged to be in the top 5% of all the grants reviewed at
that time. Given the time and resources, Blalock and his colleagues were able to
sequence the peptides which were found to be authentic.80 Other investigators
began to pay attention and the study of neuroendocrine-immune system
communication took another giant step. Today, it is accepted that brain peptides
and their receptors exist within the immune system and that the products of an
activated immune system function as neurotransmitters. Thus, the scientific
pariahs became heroes (apparently, they have not yet experienced unreferenced
descriptions of these phenomena prefaced by the phrase, “As we have long
expected…”). The process, agonizing at times, was rewarding and intellectually
stimulating, but, as Blalock puts it: the scientific enterprise would be more
enjoyable if the scientific community recognized that “science is about unexpected
Another critical link between the brain and the immune system was forged by
David Felten. He and his colleagues brought anatomical, neurochemical, receptor-
binding, and in vitro and in vivo immunological techniques to bear on this
relationship and provided unequivocal evidence that sympathetic noradrenergic
nerve fibers signal cells of the immune system and are capable of evoking major
changes in their responsiveness. Again, it was a serendipitous observation that
altered the direction of Felten’s research.
In 1980, Felten was examining a section of rodent spleen with fluorescence
histochemistry for catecholamines to distinguish arterial and venous patterns of
smooth muscle innervation. He saw and reported extensive networks of
noradrenergic sympathetic nerve fibers among T cells in the white pulp, and was
confused about why this had not been described in the past.81 Felten had always
looked at interactions among neuronal systems in a non-traditional fashion. From
his early work at MIT as an undergraduate in Walle J.H. Nauta’s laboratory, he
was fascinated with integrative regulatory neuronal systems. His unexpected
observation of sympathetic noradrenergic nerve fibers in apparent direct contact
with lymphocytes and macrophages thus fell on fertile ground. He and his
colleagues proceeded to show that these nerve fibers were localized in precise
compartments of both primary (thymus, bone marrow) and secondary (spleen,
lymph nodes) lymphoid organs,81-84 and formed close, synaptic-like neuro-
effector junctions with T lymphocytes and macrophages.85
Felten recalls that his early findings were ridiculed by many immunologists
and viewed with disbelief as “minor aberrations,” at best. However, with
characteristic energy and persistence, he and his collaborators spent several years
investigating and demonstrating that these noradrenergic nerve fibers fulfilled the
criteria for neurotransmission with cells of the immune system with thymus,
spleen, and lymph nodes as targets. In a detailed developmental study, it was
shown that these nerve fibers formed these close contacts with lymphocytes early
in ontogeny, and appeared to influence early immunological development and
compartmentation.86 At the other end of the lifespan, sympathetic nerve fibers in
secondary lymphoid organs were found to diminish markedly with age.87 Felten
has proposed that this loss contributes to immunosenescence, particularly to
diminished T cell functions, especially TH1 (cell-mediated) responses. In other
recent work, Felten’s laboratory demonstrated that local denervation of adrenergic
sympathetic nerves from draining lymph nodes in autoimmune disease-susceptible
rats enhanced joint inflammation and bone erosion in adjuvant-induced arthritis,
while selective denervation of substance P nerve fibers from such draining lymph
nodes protected the rats from joint pathology.88 Such findings substantiate the
functional importance of nerves supplying lymphoid organs.
In 1983, Felten was awarded a prestigious John D. and Catherine T.
MacArthur Foundation Prize Fellowship at the early stage of his work in neural-
immune interactions. Parenthetically, this was one of the Foundation’s few
ventures into psychoneuroimmunology. Several discussions and conferences in
the early days held out the prospect that such new, interdisciplinary research
would meet the original criteria for MacArthur Foundation support: innovative
research that would face difficulties in finding support from within traditional
federal funding agencies. Perhaps, however, it was too soon; purportedly, the
advice received by the Foundation at that time was that psychoneuroimmunology
wasn’t going anywhere. David and Suzanne Felten, however, were going to the
Department of Neurobiology and Anatomy at the University of Rochester School
of Medicine and Dentistry to team up with Bob Ader, Nick Cohen and Sandy
Livnat to develop interdisciplinary programs of research and research training.
In demonstrating a major role for sympathetic noradrenergic nerve fibers in
regulating immune functions, Felten and his colleagues provided evidence for a
direct, “hard-wired” connection between the CNS and the immune system. This
connection has since been shown to be a major route for behavioral influences and
for central cytokine influences on immune function. For Felten, the demonstration
of direct neural signaling of cells of the immune system opens up several
completely new directions for research. It is now possible to seek the chemical
and receptor-mediated mechanisms by which behavioral and other CNS influences
on immune responses are achieved. He and his colleagues are pursuing the use of
neurotransmitter agonists and antagonists to specifically manipulate sites of
initiation of immune responses, development and regulation of effector cell
functions, and modulation of effector cell functions at diverse sites. Felten’s work
is a cornerstone of a mechanistic understanding of the signaling between the
nervous and immune systems and provides a basis for understanding the complex
systemic integration among behavioral processes, the brain, and
Thus, it was during the 1970s and early 80s that independent lines of research,
derived as much from the personal experiences and imagination of the
investigators as from a logic dictated by different disciplinary perspectives, began
converging on the theme that the immune system was part of a larger, integrated
mechanism of homeostatic processes serving the survival interests of the
individual. For whatever reasons — and despite overt and covert resistance —
this was evidently the right stuff at the right time! A new picture of
immunoregulatory processes was emerging that promised a new understanding of
the functions of other narrowly conceptualized systems and a new appreciation of
the multi-determined etiology of pathophysiological states. A paradigm shift was
occurring and, as a result of the nearly twenty years of research precipitated by the
above findings, it is no longer possible to study immunoregulatory processes as an
independent function of the immune system. The research initiated by these
investigators were giant steps and, despite the fact that they originated from
different perspectives, they had a common effect. There were earlier, isolated
studies, but most of the current research in the field derives directly or indirectly
from these seminal studies. These were enabling studies in the sense that they
raised questions and, further, legitimized questions that had not been asked before.
And if these questions — and, sometimes, the questioners — were ridiculed,
another almost universal experience, the evidence was, first, compelling, and then
overwhelming. Thus, as Schopenhauer observed, “All truth passes through three
stages. First it is ridiculed. Second it is violently opposed. Third it is accepted as
being self evident.” This has almost become a cliche´, yet a recent textbook in
immunology89 devotes a section to neuroendocrine-immune system relationships
and concludes that, “Clinical and experimental psychoneuroimmunology studies
to date confirm the long-standing belief that the immune system does not function
completely autonomously.” (Italics added).
It is the research conducted during the past 20 years that probably
accounts for 95 per cent or more of what is now known about the relationships
among behavioral, neural and endocrine, and immune processes of adaptation3
and led to the general (and sometimes still begrudging) acknowledgment that, like
other physiological processes operating to protect the organism, the immune
system is part of an integrated system of adaptive processes and is thus subject to
some regulation by the brain. Two pathways link the brain with the immune
system: autonomic nervous system activity and neuroendocrine outflow via the
pituitary. Both routes provide biologically active molecules which are perceived
by the immune system via cell surface or internal receptors on the surface of
lymphocytes, monocytes/macrophages and granulocytes. Thus, all
immunoregulatory processes take place within a neuroendocrine milieu that is
demonstrably sensitive to the influence of the individual’s perception of and
response to events occurring in the external world.
Conversely, we have learned that activation of the immune system
is accompanied by changes in hypothalamic, autonomic, and endocrine processes,
and by changes in behavior. For example, cytokines influence activation of the
hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal (HPA) axis — and, in turn, are influenced by
glucocorticoid secretion.70 The potential interaction of neuroendocrine and
immune processes is further magnified by the fact that cells of the immune system
activated by immunogenic stimuli are capable of producing a variety of
neuropeptides.90 Thus, the exchange of information between the brain and the
Based on the above, it is hardly surprising that behavioral factors are capable
of modifying immune function or that activation of the immune system would
have consequences for behavior. The Pavlovian conditioning of the suppression
or enhancement of immune responses51 and, conversely, the conditioning of the
physiological effects of cytokines91 both reflect adaptive immunoregulatory
processes. The majority of the behavioral research, derived in large measure from
the work of Hans Selye, has addressed the immunologic effects of stressful
experiences. Early studies concentrated on the immunosuppressive effects of
adrenal gland activation. These pharmacologic and physiologic studies were
complemented by the behavioral studies of Rasmussen’s group in the 1950s, by
Friedman and Ader and by Solomon in the 1960s and by a host of others,
primarily physiologists, during this same time period.92,93 There was not a lot of
research of this kind from the late ‘60s until the publication of
Psychoneuroimmunology.2 In the 1980s, however, “stress and immune function”
was revived and, armed with a modern technology, became the dominant theme of
the behavioral component of psychoneuroimmunology.
Human studies of the immunologic changes associated with emotional states
and stressful life experiences also took shape in the 1980s. Stimulated by a
description of some of the immunologic effects of sudden bereavement,94
researchers began to address the effects of losses (e.g., the death of a spouse) and
of affective states, particularly, depression, on immunity. For example, Marvin
Stein, then Chair of the Department of Psychiatry at the Mt. Sinai Hospital and
Medical Center in New York, had, during the 1960s, been actively involved in
studies of the effects of hypothalamic lesions and stimulation on anaphylactic
reactions in guinea pigs.95 Like Solomon, Stein returned to
psychoneuroimmunology in the 1980s with a program of animal research on the
immunologic effects of stressful experiences and a program of human studies of
the immunologic changes associated with loss and with depression. In this, Stein
was able to engage an interdisciplinary team of young investigators (and to
stimulate the interest of several others) who now have psychoneuroimmunology
Similarly, the unique team of Janice Kiecolt-Glaser, a psychologist, and
Ronald Glaser who, as Chair of a Department of Medical Microbiology and
Immunology, entered the field with considerable apprehension, initiated an
extensive series of studies that began with the effects of examinations in medical
students on changes in immunity.96 Glaser, like many others, became convinced
of the role of behavioral factors in the modulation of immunity only when he
found such relationships in his own data. Although a common event in the life of
students, examination periods were found to be reliably associated with a general
depression of immune function including, as a consequence, an elevation in
antibodies to Epstein-Barr virus. These studies were directly and indirectly
responsible for a reemergence of animal and human studies by behavioral
scientists and by immunologists on the effects of stressful life experiences on
immune function and susceptibility to infectious diseases.
In animals and in humans, a variety of psychosocial events interpreted as being
stressful to the organism are capable of influencing a variety of immune responses.
It is now clear, however, that different “stressors” have different effects on some
constant outcome measure and that the same “stressor” can have different effects
on different outcome measures. The direction, magnitude and duration of stress-
induced alterations of immunity are influenced by: (a) the quality and quantity of
stressful stimulation; (b) the capacity of the individual to cope effectively with
stressful events; (c) the quality and quantity of immunogenic stimulation; (d) the
temporal relationship between stressful stimulation and immunogenic stimulation;
(e) sampling times and the particular aspect of immune function (or compartment)
chosen for measurement; (f) the experiential history of the individual and the
existing social and environmental conditions upon which stressful and
immunogenic stimulation are superimposed; (g) a variety of host factors such as
species, strain, age, sex, and nutritional state; and (h) interactions among these
several variables. This listing of relevant variables identified in recent research
paraphrases the variables identified from a much earlier analysis of the effects of
stressful life experiences on behavioral and physiological responses and on
susceptibility to disease.17,97 Indeed, prospective as well as retrospective studies
in animals and humans have also shown that, depending on interactions among the
qualitative and quantitative nature of the environmental demands and the
pathophysiologic process, the experimental procedures, and a variety of host
factors, stressful experiences can alter the host’s defense mechanisms thereby
altering susceptibility to bacterial and viral infections, modifying the
neuroinvasiveness of normally non-neurovirulent strains of virus, or allowing an
otherwise inconsequential exposure to a pathogen to develop into clinical
The behavioral and emotional states that attend the perception of and the effort
to adapt to environmental circumstances are accompanied by complex patterns of
neuroendocrine changes. That the neural and endocrine patterns associated with
behavioral and emotional states are capable of modulating immune functions lends
credence to the hypothesis that changes in immune function constitute an
important mediator of the pathophysiological effects of stressful life experiences.
This chain of psychophysiological events may not yet have been firmly
established, but, as this volume attests, the possibility is attracting renewed
attention and the data are providing evidence of the relevance of psychosocially-
induced alterations in immune function for differences in the susceptibility to and
psychoneuroimmunology today? It’s still working out some
adolescent problems and on its way to young adulthood. It has not yet achieved
the maturity of neuroendocrinology or the more recent psychoneuroendocrinology
— and it has not been granted the scientific respectability it has earned and to
which it is entitled. Still, psychoneuroimmunology continues to grow. Brain, Behavior and Immunity began publishing in 1987 and there are now two other
journals devoted to the area. Research reports are now considered (and solicited)
for publication in a variety of peer reviewed journals in immunology, psychology
and in the neurosciences, including endocrinology. There are now two
international societies and papers in the field occupy increasing blocks of time at
the meetings of other scientific societies. There has been an increase in the
number of students (including M.D./Ph.D. candidates) from psychology,
immunology and the neurosciences interested in working in the area, and there has
been an increase in the number of funded research and research training grants.
There has also been a proliferation of edited volumes that address various aspects
of the field (e.g., Psychoneuroimmunology, Stress and Infection).
Psychoneuroimmunology is, perhaps, the most recent example of an
interdisciplinary field that has developed and now prospers by exploring and
tilling fertile territories secreted by the arbitrary and illusory boundaries of the
biomedical sciences. Disciplinary boundaries and the bureaucracies they spawned
are biological fictions that can restrict imagination and the transfer and application
of technologies. They lend credence to Werner Heisenberg’s assertion that “What
we observe is not nature itself, but nature exposed to our method of questioning.”
(p. 81).100 Our own language, too, must change. The signal molecules of the
nervous and immune systems are expressed and perceived by both systems.
Therefore, it may no longer be appropriate to speak of “neurotransmitters” and
“immunotransmitters.” Also, to speak of links or channels of communication
between the nervous and immune systems perpetuates the myth that these are
discrete systems (or disciplines). On the contrary, the evidence indicates that
relationships between so-called “systems” are as important and, perhaps, more
important than relationships within “systems;” that so-called “systems” are critical
components of a single, integrated network of homeostatic mechanisms. To the
extent that the problems chosen for study and innovative research strategies to
address these problems derive from conceptual and theoretical positions, these are
More substantively, research conducted over the past several years has resulted
in a recognition and appreciation of the interactions among behavioral, neural,
endocrine, and immune processes. Indeed, there has been a paradigm shift in the
attempt to understand immunoregulatory function. The innervation of lymphoid
organs and the availability of neurotransmitters for interactions with cells of the
immune system add a new dimension to our understanding of the
microenvironment in which immune responses take place. Similarly, the
interaction between pituitary-, endocrine organ-, and lymphocyte-derived
hormones that define the neuroendocrine milieu in which immune responses occur
adds another level of complexity to the analysis of the cellular interactions that
drive immune responses. Collectively, these relationships provide the foundation
for previously observed behaviorally-induced alterations in immune function and
for immunologically based changes in behavior. They may also provide the
means by which psychosocial factors and the emotional states that accompany the
perception and response to stressful life experiences influence the development
and progression of infectious, autoimmune and neoplastic disease.
I am grateful to Drs. Hugo Besedovsky, J. Edwin Blalock, John Hadden, and
George Solomon for providing for me a brief written description of their
perspectives on psychoneuroimmunology and for their comments and corrections
of an earlier draft of this paper. I am also indebted to Dr. Sherman M. Melinkoff,
Professor Emeritus of Medicine and former Dean of the UCLA School of
Medicine for his personal reflections of A. Fred Rasmussen. Thanks are also due
to my colleagues, Drs. David Felten and Nicholas Cohen who contributed material
for this essay. The responsibility for the selection of these particular perspectives,
the editorializing, and any remaining errors are my own.
Preparation of this paper was supported by a Research Scientist Award (K05
MH06318) from the National Institute of Mental Health.
*As I write this chapter, it seems evident that the financial support for research in
psychoneuroimmunology, despite its successes, will face serious difficulties over
the next several years (but that’s a different chapter).
1. Ader, R., Presidential address: Psychosomatic and psychoimmunologic
research, Psychosom. Med., 42, 307, 1980.
Ader, R., Ed., Psychoneuroimmunology, Academic Press, New York, 1981.
Ader, R., Felten, D.L., Cohen, N., Eds, Psychoneuroimmunology, Second Edition, Academic Press, New York, 1991.
Metal’nikov, S., Chorine, V., Role des reflexes conditonnels dans l’imunite,
Ann. Inst. Pasteur, 40, 893, 1926.
5. Hull, C.L., The factor of conditioned reflex, in A Handbook of General Experimental Psychology, C. Murchison,Ed., Worcester, Clark Univer.,
6. Kopeloff, N., Kopeloff, L.M., Raney, M.E., The nervous system and
antibody production, Psychiat Quart, 7, 84, 1933.
Bacteriology in Neuropsychiatry, Charles C Thomas,
Ader, R., A historical account of conditioned immunobiologic responses, in
Psychoneuroimmunology, Ader, R., Ed., Academic Press, New York, 1981,
9. Rasmussen, A.F., Jr., Marsh, J.T., Brill, N.Q., Increased susceptibility to
herpes simplex in mice subjected to avoidance-learning stress or restraint,
Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med., 96, 183, 1957.
Johnson, T., Lavender, J.F., Hultin, F., Rasmussen, A.F., Jr., The influence of
avoidance-learning stress on resistance to Coxsackie B virus in mice, J.
11. Jensen, M.M., Rasmussen, A.F., Jr., Stress and susceptibility to viral
infection. I. Response of adrenals, liver, thymus, spleen and peripheral
leukocyte counts to sound stress, J. Immunol., 90, 17, 1963.
Marsh, J.T., Lavender, J.F., Chang, S.S., Rasmussen, A.F., Jr., Poliomyelitis
in monkeys: Decreased susceptibility after avoidance stress, Science, 1450,
Chang, S.S., Rasmussen, A.F., Jr., Effect of stress on susceptibility of mice to
polyoma virus infection, Bacteriol. Proc., 64, 134, 1964.
Chang, S.S., Rasmussen, A.F., Jr., Stress-induced suppression of interferon
production in virus-infected mice, Nature, 205, 623, 1965.
Yamada, A., Jensen, M.M., Rasmussen, A.F., Jr., Stress and susceptibility to
viral infections. III. Antibody response and viral retention during avoidance
learning stress, Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med., 116, 677, 1964.
Friedman, S.B., Ader, R., Glasgow, L.A., Effects of psychological stress in
adult mice inoculated with Coxsackie B viruses, Psychosom. Med., 27, 361,
Friedman, S.B., Glasgow, L.A., Ader, R., Psychosocial factors modifying
host resistance to experimental infections, Ann. NY Acad. Sci. , 164, 381,
Solomon, G.F., Stress and antibody response in rats, Int. Arch. Allergy, 35,
Solomon, G.F., Emotions and personality factors in the onset and course of
autoimmune disease, particularly rheumatoid arthritis, in
Psychoneuroimmunology, Ader, R., Ed., Academic Press, New York, 1981,
20. Solomon, G.F., Moos, R.H., Fessel, W.J., Morgan, E.E., Globulins and
behavior in schizophrenia, Int. J. Neuropsychiat., 2, 20, 1966.
Solomon, G.F., Moos, R.H., The relationship of personality to the presence
of rheumatoid factor in asymptomatic relatives of patients with rheumatoid
arthritis, Psychsom. Med., 27, 350, 1965.
Good, R.A., Experimental models in systemic lupus erythematosus, Arthr.
23. Dixon, F.J., Feldman, J, Vasquez, J., Immunology an pathogenesis of
experimental serum sickness, in Cellular and Humoral Aspects of Hypersensitive States, Lawrence, S.H., Ed., Hoeber, New York, 1959.
Solomon, G.F., Moos, R.H., Emotions, immunity and disease: A speculative
theoretical integration, Arch. Gen. Psychiat., 11, 657, 1964.
25. Solomon, G.F., Merigan, T., Levine, S., Variations in adrenal cortical
hormones within physiological ranges and interferon production in mice,
Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med., 126, 74, 1967.
26. Solomon, G.F., Levine, S., Kraft, J.K., Early experience and immunity,
Amkraut, A.A., Solomon, G.F., Stress and murine sarcoma virus (Maloney)-
induced tumors, Cancer Res., 32, 1428, 1972.
Amkraut, A.A., Solomon, G.F., Kaspar, P., Purdue, A., Effect of stress and of
hormonal intervention on the graft versus host response, Adv. Exp. Med.
Amkraut, A.A., Solomon, G.F., Kraemer, H.C., Stress, early experience, and
adjuvant-induced arthritis in the rat, Psychosom. Med., 33, 203, 1971.
30. Solomon, G.F., Temoshuk, L., O’Leary, A., Zich, J., An intensive
immunologic study of long-surviving persons with AIDS: Plot work,
background studies, hypotheses, and methods, Ann. N.Y. Acad. Sci., 496,
31. Szentivanyi, A., The beta adrenergic theory of the atopic abnormality in
bronchial asthma, J. Allergy, 42, 203, 1968.
Szentivanyi, A., Filipp, G., Anaphylaxis and the nervous system. II., Ann.
Szentivanyi, A., Szekely, J., Anaphylaxis and the nervous system. IV., Ann.
34. Hadden, J.W., Hadden, E.M., Middleton, E., Lymphocyte blast
transformation. I. Demonstration of adrenergic receptors in human peripheral
lymphocytes, J. Cell. Immunol., 1, 583, 1970.
Bourne, H., Melmon, K., Liechtenstein, L., Histamine augments leukocyte
adenosine 3’,5’- monophosphate and blocks antigenic histamine release,
Hadden, J.W., Hadden, E.M., Good, R.A., Alpha adrenergic stimulation of
glucose uptake in the human erythrocyte, lymphocyte and lymphoblast, Exp.
Hadden, J.W., Hadden, E.M., Middleton, E., Good, R.A., Lymphocyte blast
transformation. II. The mechanisms of action of alpha adrenergic receptor
effects, Int. Arch. Allergy Appl. Immunol., 40, 526, 1971.
Coffey, R.G., Hadden, E.M., Hadden, J.W., Norepinephrine stimulation of
membrane ATPase in human lymphocytes, Endocrinol. Res. Comm., 12, 179,
Hadden, J.W., Hadden, E.M., Haddox, M., Goldberg N.D., Guanosine cyclic
3’5’-monophosphate: A possible intracellular mediator of mitogenic
influences in lymphocytes, Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 69, 3024, 1972.
40. Strom, T.B., Disseroth, B., Morganroth, J., Carpenter, C, Merrill, J.,
Alteration of the cytotoxic action of sensitized lymphocytes by cholinergic
agents and activators of adenylate cyclase, Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., 69, 2995,
41. Hadden, J.W., Hadden, E.M., Meetz, G., Good, R.A., Haddox, M.K.,
Goldberg, N.D., Cyclic GMP in cholinergic and mitogenic modulation of
lymphocyte metabolism and proliferation, Fed. Proc., 32, 1022, 1973.
Hadden, J.W., Hadden, E.M., Coffey, R.G., Johnson, E.M., Johnson, L.D.,
Cyclic GMP and lymphocyte activation, in Immune Recognition, Rosenthal,
A., Ed., Academic Press, New York, 1975, 359.
Good, R. A., Foreword: Interactions of the body’s major networks, in
Psychoneuroimmunology, Ader, R., Ed., Academic Press, New York, 1981,
Hadden, J.W., Thymic endocrinology, Int. J. Immunopharmac., 14, 345,
45. Saha, A.R. Hadden, E.M., Sosa, M., and Hadden, J.W., Thymus in
neuroendocrine perspective, in Nitric Oxide: Brain and Immune System,
Moncada, S., Nistico, G., and Higgs, E.A., Eds., Portland Press, England,
Ader, R., Letter to the Editor: Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression,
47. Ader, R., Cohen, N., Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression,
48. Rogers, M.P., Reich, P. Strom, T.B., and Carpenter, C.B., Behaviorally
conditioned immunosuppression: Replication of a recent study, Psychosom.
49. Ader, R., Cohen, N., CNS-immune system interactions: Conditioning
phenomena, Behav. Brain Sci., 8, 379, 1985.
Ader, R., Cohen, N., The influence of conditioning on immune responses, in
Psychoneuroimmunology, Second Edition, Ader, R., Felten, D.L., Cohen, N.,
Eds., Academic Press, New York, 611, 1991.
Ader, R., & Cohen, N., Psychoneuroimmunology: Conditioning and stress,
Ann. Rev. Psychol., 44, 53, 1993.
52. Ader, R., Kelly, K., Moynihan, J., Grota, L.J., Cohen, N., Conditioned
enhancement of antibody production using antigen as the unconditioned
stimulus, Brain Behav. Immun., 7, 334, 1993.
53. Gorczynski, R.M., Macrae, S., and Kennedy, M., Conditioned immune
response associated with allogeneic skin grafts in mice, J. Immunol., 129,
54. Ader, R., Cohen, N., Behaviorally conditioned immunosuppression and
murine systemic lupus erythematosus, Science, 214, 1534, 1982.
55. Gorczynski, R.M., Conditioned enhancement of skin allografts in mice,
Brain Behav. Immun., 4, 85, 1990.
56. Grochowicz, P., Schedlowski, M., Husband, A.J., King, M.G., Hibberd,
A.D., Bowen, K.M., Behavioral conditioning prolongs heart allograft
survival in rats, Brain Behav. Immun., 5, 349, 1991.
Olness, K, Ader, R., Conditioning as an adjunct in the pharmacotherapy of
lupus erythematosus, J. Develop. Behav. Ped., 13,124, 1992.
Besedovsky, H.O., Delay in skin allograft rejection in rats grafted with fetal
adrenal glands, Experientia, 26, 697, 1971.
Besedovsky, H.O., Sorkin, E., Keller, M., and Mueller, J., Hormonal changes
during the immune response, Proc. Soc. Exp. Biol. Med., 150, 466, 1975.
60. Besedovsky, H.O., and Sorkin, E., Network of immune-neuroendocrine
interactions, Clin. Exp. Immunol., 27, 1, 1977.
Besedovsky, H.O., Sorkin, E., Felix,. R. and Haas, H., Hypothalamic changes
during the immune response, Europ. J. Immunol., 7, 323, 1977.
62. Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A.E., and Sorkin, E., Antigenic competition
between horse and sheep red blood cells as a hormone-dependent
phenomenon, Clin. Exp. Immunol., 37, 106, 1979.
63.Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A.E., Sorkin, E., DaPrada, M., and Keller, H.A.,
Immunoregulation mediated by the sympathetic nervous system, Cell.
del Rey, A., Besedovsky, H.O., Sorkin, E., DaPrada, M., and Bondiolotti, P.,
Sympathetic immunoregulation: Difference between high- and low-responder
animals, Am. J. Physiol., 242, R30, 1982.
del Rey, A., Besedovsky, H.O., Sorkin, E., DaPrada, M., and Arrenbrecht, S.,
Immunoregulation mediated by the sympathetic nervous system. II., Cell.
Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A.E., Sorkin, E., DaPrada, M., Burri, R., and
Honegger, C., The immune response evokes changes in brain noradrenergic
neurons, Science, 221, 564, 1983.
Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A.E., and Sorkin, E., Lymphokine containing
supernatants from Con A-stimulated cells increase corticosterone blood
levels, J. Immunol., 126, 385, 1981.
Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A.E., Sorkin, E., Lotz, W., and Schwulera, U.,
Lymphoid cells produce an immunoregulatory glucocorticoid increasing
factors (GIF) acting through the pituitary gland, Clin Exp. Immunol., 59,
69. Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A., Sorkin, E., and Dinarello, C.A.,
Immunoregulatory feedback between interleukin-1 and glucocorticoid
hormones, Science, 233, 652, 1986.
Berkenbosch, J., Van Oers, J., del Rey, A., Tilders, F., and Besedovky, H.O.,
Corticotropin-releasing factor-producing neurons in the rat activated by
interleukin-1, Science, 238, 524, 1987.
Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A. & Normann, S., Host endocrine responses
during tumor growth, in Immunity to Cancer II., Mitchell, M.S., Ed., Alan
72. deRijk, R., van Rooujen, N., Besedovsky, H.O., del Rey, A., and
Berkenbosch, F., Selective depletion of macrophages prevents pituitary-
adrenal activation in response to subpyrogenic, but not to pyrogenic, doses of
bacterial endotoxin in rats, Endocrinology, 129, 330, 1991.
Besedovsky, H.O. and del Rey, A., Mechanism of virus-induced stimulation
of the hypothalamic-pituitary-adrenal axis, J. Steroid Biochem., 34, 235,
Cohen, N. & Ader, R., Antibodies and learning: A new dimension, in The Immune System, Vol. 1, Steinberg, C., & Lefkovits, I., Eds., Karger, Basel,
Blalock, J.E. and Smith, E.M., Human leukocyte interferon: Structural and
biological relatedness to adrenocorticotropic hormone and endorphins, Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., USA, 77, 5972, 1980.
76. Blalock, J.E. and Harp. C., Interferon and adrenocorticotropic hormone
induction of steroidogenesis, melanogenesis and antiviral activity, Arch.
77. Smith, E.M. and Blalock, J.E., Human lymphocyte production of
corticotropin- and endorphin-like substances: Association with leukocyte
interferon, Proc. Nat. Acad. Sci., USA, 78, 7530, 1981.
Blalock, J.E., The immune system as a sensory organ, J. Immunol., 132,
79. Blalock, J.E., The syntax of immune-neuroendocrine communication,
Smith, E.M., Galin, F.S., LeBoeuf, R.D., Coppenhaver, D.H., Harbour, D.V.,
and Blalock, J.E., Nucleotide and amino acid sequence of lymphocyte-
derived corticotropin: Endotoxin induction of a truncated peptide, Proc Nat.
Williams, J.M., Peterson, R.G., Shea, P.A., Schmedtje, J.F., Bauer, D.C., and
Felten, D.L., Sympathetic innervation of murine thymus and spleen:
Evidence for a functional link between the nervous and immune system,
Brain Res. Bulletin, 6, 83, 1981.
82. Livnat, S., Felten, S.Y., Carlson, S.L., Bellinger, D.L., and Felten, D.L.,
Involvement of peripheral and central catecholamine systems in neural-
immune interactions, J. Neuroimmunology, 10, 5, 1985.
83. Felten, D.L., Ackerman, K.D., Wiegand, S.J., and Felten, S.Y.,
Noradrenergic sympathetic innervation of the spleen: I. Nerve fibers
associate with lymphocytes and macrophages in specific compartments of the
splenic white pulp, J. Neurosci. Res, 18, 28, 1987.
Felten, D.L., Felten, S.Y., Bellinger, D.L., Carlson, S.L., Ackerman, K.D.,
Madden, K.S., Olschowka, J.A., and Livnat, S., Noradrenergic sympathetic
neural interactions with the immune system: Structure and function,
Felten, S.Y., and Olschowka, J.A., Noradrenergic sympathetic innervation of
the spleen: II. Tyrosine hydroxylase (TH)-positive nerve terminals form
synaptic-like contacts on lymphocytes in the splenic white pulp, J. Neurosci.
Ackerman, K.D., Felten, S.Y., Dijkstra, C.D., Livnat, S., and Felten, D.L.,
Parallel development of noradrenergic innervation and cellular
compartmentation in the rat spleen, Exp. Neurol., 103, 239, 1989.
Bellinger, D.L., Ackerman, K.D., Felten, S.Y., Pulera, M., and Felten, D.L.,
A longitudinal study of age-related loss of noradrenergic nerves and
lymphoid cells in the rat spleen, Exp. Neurol., 116, 295, 1992.
Felten, D.L., Felten, S.Y., Bellinger, D.L., and Lorton, D., Noradrenergic and
peptidergic innervation of secondary lymphoid organs: Role in experimental
rheumatoid arthritis, Europ. J. Clin. Invest., 22, Suppl. 1, 37, 1992.
Stites, D.P., and Terr, A.I., Basis and Clinical Immunology, Seventh Edition,
Appleton & Lange, Norwalk, 190, 1991.
Weigant, A. and Blalock, J.E., Role of neuropeptides in the
bidirectional communication between the immune and neuroendocrine
systems, in Neuropeptides and Immunoregulation, Scharrer, B., Smith, E.M.,
& Stefano, G.B., Eds., Springer-Verlag, Berlin, 1994, 14.
Dyck, D,. Janz, L., Osachuk, T.A.G., Falk, J., Labinsky, J. and Greenberg,
A.H., The Pavlovian conditioning of IL-1-induced glucocorticoid secretion,Brain Behav. Immun., 4, 93, 1990.
Monjan, A.A., Stress and immunologic competence: Studies in animals, in
Psychoneuroimmunology, Ader, R., Ed., Academic Press, New York, 1981,
Ader, R., and Cohen, N., Behavior and the immune system, in Handbook of Behavioral Medicine Research, Gentry, W.D., Ed., Guilford Press, New
Bartrop, R.W., Luckhurst, E., Lazarus, L., Kiloh, L.G., Penny, R., Depressed
lymphocyte function after bereavement, Lancet , i, 834, 1977.
Stein, M., Schleifer, S.J., Keller, S.E., Hypothalamic influences on immune
responses, in Psychoneuroimmunology, Ader, R., Ed., Academic Press, New
Glaser, R., Rice, J., Sheridan, J., Fertel, R., Stout, J., Speicher, C., Pinsky, D.,
Kotur, M., Post, A., Beck, M., Kiecolt-Glaser, J.A., Stress-related immune
suppression: Health implications, Brain Behav. Immun., 1, 7, 1987.
97. Ader, R., The influence of psychological factors on disease susceptibility in
animals, in The Husbandry of Laboratory Animals, Conalty, M.L., Ed.,
98. Cohen, S. and Williamson, G., Stress and infectious disease in humans,
99. Sheridan, J.F., Dobbs, C., Brown, D., Zwilling, B.,
Psychoneuroimmunology: Stress effects on pathogenesis and immunity
during infection, Clin. Microbiol. Rev., 7, 200, 1994.
Heisenberg, W., Physics and Philosophy, the Revolution in Modern Science,
PathMDTM: Board Review Letter Author: Jennifer Linder, DO & Lynda Bradshaw, MT (ASCP) Microbiology – Part 1 1. You receive plates from a respiratory specimen that exhibit no growth on a blood agar plate and growth of small gray colonies on a chocolate plate. You perform a gram stain, which reveals small, pleomorphic gram-negative rods. Based on these results you conclude that this o
Innocent Man by John Grisham The Summer Reading program, sponsored by the English Department at Padua Franciscan High School, is designed to entice students to read and recognize notable contemporary literature. We wish to engage student interest, encourage critical thinking, and challenge our students as readers. As such, some titles on the list may contain sensitive material and deal