How somatic cortical maps differ in autistic and typical brainsMehmet Akif Coskuna, Larry Varghesea, Stacy Reddochc, Eduardo M. Castillod,Deborah A. Pearsonc, Katherine A. Lovelandc, Andrew C. Papanicolaoudand Bhavin R. Shetha,b
The comorbidity of ‘core characteristics’ and sensorimotor
abnormalities in autism implies abnormalities in brain
Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins.
development of a general and pervasive nature and
atypical organization of sensory cortex. By using
magnetoencephalography, we examined the cortical
Keywords: autism, cortical organization, magnetoencephalography,
response to passive tactile stimulation of the thumb and
index finger of the dominant hand and lip of the individuals
with autism spectrum disorder and typically developing
aDepartment of Electrical and Computer Engineering, bCenter forNeuroEngineering and Cognitive Systems, University of Houston, Houston,
persons. The distance between the cortical representations
Departments of cPsychiatry and Behavioral Sciences and dPediatrics, The
of thumb and the lip was significantly larger in the autism
University of Texas Health Science Center at Houston, Houston, Texas, USA
group than in typicals. Moreover, in cortex, the thumb is
Correspondence to Bhavin R. Sheth, PhD, University of Houston, Houston,
typically closer to the lip than the index finger. This was not
observed in persons with autism. Our findings are arguably
Tel: + 1 713 743 4935; fax: + 1 713 743 4444; e-mail: brsheth@uh.edu
the first demonstration of abnormality in sensory
organization in the brains of persons with
Received 25 September 2008 accepted 17 October 2008
individuals in the autism group met our research criteria
Children with autistic spectrum disorders often have
for an autism spectrum disorder, as determined by using
unusual reactions to certain sensory stimuli. Across
the Autism Diagnostic Observation Schedule [10] and
reports, 44–88% of individuals with autism have abnormal
Autism Diagnostic Interview, Revised [11] administered
sensitivity to tactile stimuli [1]. It is possible that
by trained clinicians. Five individuals in the autism group
abnormal responsivity to touch is a sensory perceptual
were clinically classified as pervasive developmental
problem (see Ref. [2]) or stems from some deviation in
disorder-not otherwise specified, one as Asperger syn-
sensory cortical organization. Of importance, these
drome, and the remaining eight as autistic disorder. Two
unusual behaviors appear early in development during
persons with autism and three typicals were female.
the time the somatic map is being formed [3]. The
Potential participants were excluded if there was
association of sensorimotor differences and social-
evidence of brain injury, seizure disorder, or neurotropic
emotional impairments in autism suggests that both
infection or disease, or had a history of identified severe
areas of function could be affected by abnormalities in
psychopathology, such as bipolar disorder, schizophrenia,
brain development of a more general nature that have
or behavior problems severe enough to make accurate
the potential to adversely affect development in
and reliable testing difficult. All participants were right
multiple systems. As a first step to elucidate this relation-
handed as determined by the Edinburgh Handedness
ship, we investigated somatotopy in the brains of young
Inventory [12]. All in the autism group were high
functioning; full-scale IQs (FSIQs) and verbal IQs (VIQs)
(MEG). Somatotopic maps of various body parts
derived from the Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelli-
(e.g. lip, trunk, and shoulder) and even of the individual
gence [13] were greater than 85 (mean ± 1 SEM: FSIQ,
fingers of a single hand have been obtained in past studies
105 ± 5; VIQ, 103 ± 5; and performance IQ, 105 ± 4).
by using MEG [4–6]. In addition, MEG has also
Prior informed consent was obtained from all participants,
been used to investigate the biological basis of autism
or participants and their parents, under a protocol
approved by the University of Texas Health ScienceCenter-Houston and the University of Houston.
Pneumatically driven mechanical taps (1.8 kg/cm2) of
(mean ± SEM = 18.6 ± 1.0 years) and 16 typically devel-
40 ms duration (20 ms rise time) were individually
oping persons (typicals, 19.5 ± 1.0 years) participated. All
applied to the right thumb, right index finger, and the
c 2009 Wolters Kluwer Health | Lippincott Williams & Wilkins
Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
right lip (4D Neuroimaging Inc., San Diego, California,
Data from two of 14 persons with autism and three of 16
USA). Participants were informed that a pressure pulse
typicals were removed on the basis of the following two
would be delivered and that all they had to do was close
criteria: (i) the hot spots for thumb, index finger, and lip
their eyes, stay still, and relax. A training block containing
were not in line with known somatotopic order. According
five stimuli before the actual recording familiarized the
to known topography of somatosensory cortex, the hot
spot corresponding to the index finger is superior to thehot spots of the thumb and lip, and the hot spotcorresponding to the thumb is superior to that of the lip
Data acquisition and magnetoencephalography analysis
[6,15]. Among excluded participants, one or both were
All MEG recordings used a whole-head neuromagnet-
not observed; (ii) the automated dipole estimation
ometer containing an array of 248 gradiometers (Magnes
method failed to obtain a reasonable hot spot location.
WH3600, 4D Neuroimaging Inc.). The instruments were
This usually occurred because a spurious extremum
placed in a magnetically shielded and sound-attenuated
occurred in the contour map, which is the automated
room (Vacuumschmelze Gmbh & Co., KG, Hanau,
method used for dipole calculation. Excluded partici-
Germany). There were 2000 epochs of stimulation of
pants, thus, had abnormally large or small interhot spot
the index finger and lip each and 700 epochs of
distance. Manual estimation yielded reasonable hot spot
stimulation of the thumb in separate blocks. A single
locations but we did not use those estimates to maintain
epoch lasted 575 ms, and included a 120-ms prestimulus
objectivity. Of interest, the present findings are robust to
baseline. The signal was high-pass analog filtered (1.0 Hz
either method: manual estimation yielded statistically
cutoff) and the data acquired at a rate of 32 kHz
significant findings similar to the findings obtained using
were decimated to a final sampling rate of 290 Hz using
a two-stage decimating digital FIR low-pass filter (100 Hzcutoff). Any epochs that contained exaggerated moments,such as eye blinks ( > 2pT) were discarded. Portions of
the signal that were correlated to sensors placed far away
Statistical analysis used SPSS 15 for Windows (SPSS Inc.,
from the head were likely to be noise, so were subtracted
Chicago, Illinois, USA). Student’s t-tests (two-tailed) were
out. Remaining epochs were ensemble averaged.
used to examine group differences in age, IQ, head volume,and distance between the hot spots of thumb, index finger,and lip. Linear correlations between interhot spot distance
The representation of a body part in cortex, or cortical
and a host of variables related to IQ and brain volume1/3
‘hot spot,’ was the location of the earliest equivalent
were computed and analyzed for significance.
current source, or dipole, of the evoked contralateralcortical response to its tactile stimulation. To avoidexperiment bias and thereby enhance replicability and
objectivity of the process, hot spots corresponding to
Stimulation evoked a characteristic cortical response with
stimulation of the thumb, index finger, and lip were
several components at varying latencies that began with
localized in the brain by using a fully automated method
a strong response in the contralateral cortex 60–90 ms
validated earlier [14]. Only current sources that had
following the touch. The evoked cortical MEG response
a field variance greater than 94% and a localized volume
was modeled as coming from a single source or column(s)
less than 20 cm3 were accepted. The pairwise distances
in cortex. The calculations yielded the location of the
between the centers of the hotspots of the thumb, index
predominant site of activation in cortex in response to
finger, and lower lip thus obtained were compared
stimulation of the given body part, that is the cortical hot
spot. We examined the distance in cortex between thehot spots of the lip and of each of the fingers.
To estimate brain volume, we modeled the head asa rectangular prism; that is to say, each axis along the head
First, we measured the reliability of the cortical maps.
constituted a dimension of the prism. We used the
The raw data obtained from each participant were split
fiducial points and head shape digitization points to
into two equal halves, or blocks, of trials corresponding to
calculate the height, width, and length of the prism.
early and later recordings. For each block, we computed
Prism width is defined as the anteroposterior distance
the distance between the cortical representations of
between the location of the nasion on the forehead
thumb and index finger on the one hand and the lip on
(bridge of the nose) and the most posterior point at the
the other. The discrepancy between the two blocks was
back of the head; prism length is the distance between
less than 0.2 cm on average and was not significantly
the preauricular (ear) coil locations corresponding to the
different from 0 for each pairwise comparison (P > 0.8 on
external meati along the mediolateral axis; prism height is
each). Thus, our analysis indicates that reliable and stable
the distance between the most superior point on the
cortical maps were obtained, in line with past studies
conducted by us showing that somatic maps obtained
Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Somatic cortical maps in autism Coskun et al.
with MEG are reliable and highly concordant with more
was not uniform, however; the distance between the
cortical hot spots corresponding to the index fingerand lip was numerically (Fig. 1b, inset), though not
Next, we compared somatic distances between the two
significantly, smaller on average in the autism group
groups of participants by using all available data.
The distance between the representations in cortex of
P = 0.18, two-tailed t-test). The inhomogeneity in
thumb and lip was about 38% greater on average in the
somatic map extent was revealed in a clearer way,
autism group (2.26 ± 0.13 cms) compared with typical
when the ratios of the index finger–lip distance to the
(1.63 ± 0.08 cm; Fig. 1a). The difference was significant
thumb–lip distance for the two groups were compared.
[t(23) = 3.97, P < 0.001, two-tailed t-test]. This expan-
Typically in cortex, the representation of the thumb as
sion in cortical distance in the brains of the autism group
compared with that of the index finger is significantlycloser to the representation of the lip (thumb – lip/indexfinger – lip = 0.74 in our typical sample). In contrast, the
cortical representations of the thumb and index fingerwere nearly equidistant from that of the lip in our autism
group (thumb – lip/index finger – lip = 0.99). The ratio ofdistance between thumb and lip hot spots on the one handand index finger and lip on the other, that is (thumb – lip)/
(index finger – lip), in the group of typicals was significantly
different from one (0.74; P < 0.01; Fig. 1b), but not for the
autism group (0.99; P > 0.8; Fig. 1b) and the ratio between
the groups was significantly different as well (P = 0.003).
The difference in somatic map extent in autism versus
typical is not attributable to differences in brain size at
the time of the recordings. First, although our autism grouphad slightly larger brain volumes (4295 ± 112 cm3) than
the typicals (4004 ± 97 cm3), but it was not significantlyso. Second, interhot spot distance (thumb – lip) and brainsize (brain volume1/3) were not significantly correlated in
either of the two groups (R2 < 0.01, P > 0.4). Finally, even
after normalization by head size, thereby yielding a
dimensionless ratio – (thumb-lip)/(brain volume1/3) –
thumb and lip hot spots remained significantly more distant
in the autism group compared with typicals (P = 0.03).
Thus, differences in brain volume at the time of the
recordings between the two groups could not entirely
explain the larger distance between the thumb and lip
The difference in somatic map extent in autism versus
typical is also not attributable to differences in IQ. The
autism group was all high-functioning individuals with
FSIQs and VIQs greater than 85, and mean FSIQ, VIQ, andperformance IQ above 100 (average IQ). Nonetheless, our
typicals had significantly higher IQs. Of importance,however, interhot spot distance (thumb – lip) was not
significantly correlated with VIQ, performance IQ, or FSIQ
in either group (R2 < 0.1, P > 0.4 in all six cases).
Somatic map distances in autism. (a) The distance (mean ± SEM)in centimeters between the representations in somatic cortex
corresponding to the right thumb and lip in persons with autismdisorder (AD) (light gray bar) and typicals (black bar). (b) The
Cortical representations of the thumb and of the lip in
(thumb–lip)/(index finger–lip) cortical distance ratio (mean ± SEM) in
our sample of individuals with autism spectrum disorders
autism group and typicals is shown. The inset shows the distance in
were significantly more distant compared with those of
cortex between the representations, or ‘hot spots’, of index finger andlip. *P < 0.05; **P < 0.01; ***P < 0.001.
typically developing individuals. The representation ofthe lip was equidistant from the representations of the
Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
thumb and index finger in the cortex of individuals with
circuitry, are thus likely to be involved in giving rise to the
autism, in contrast to the cortex of typical individuals.
increased distance. Complicating matters further, the
This atypical nature of somatic map extent in autism is
distance between the lip and the index finger was not
among the first demonstrations of an abnormality in
larger in the Autism group compared with typical, but
sensory organization in the brains of persons with autism.
slightly smaller. In summary, the evidence in totalitysuggests that any relationship between brain growth and
Dynamic modulation of functional sensory organization in
somatic map development is likely to be complex.
somatosensory cortex has been observed but only whenthe individual is performing a motor task with cognitive
demands [16]. Participants in this study did not perform
The present findings show subtle but significant
any cognitive or motor task; therefore, differences in
deviations from normal in the organization of somatosen-
cognitive ability or motor activity between the two
sory cortex in individuals with autism, and reveal
groups, if they exist at all, are unlikely to play any role
abnormalities in brain development and cortical circuitry
in the present findings. The experimental condition in
in autism that go beyond brain areas and circuitry
this study is equivalent to a rest condition in terms of task
corresponding to autism’s ‘core characteristics’.
demand – a common baseline in neuroimaging studies. Moreover,
amplitude and latency, are remarkably robust to change
Financial disclosures: the authors report no competing
in level of attention [17]. In total, differences in
interests. The research was supported by a grant from the
attention, motor task, or cognitive demand – if they exist
National Alliance for Autism Research – AutismSpeaks
at all – are unlikely to account for abnormalities in
(BRS). M.A.C. was supported in part by a Presidential
functional sensory organization in autism we observed.
fellowship from the University of Houston.
People with autism have abnormally large heads early in
childhood, between 1 and 4 years of age; as they near
Baranek GT, Parham LD, Bodfish JW. Sensory and motor features in autism:
adulthood, the difference in head size compared with
assessment and intervention. In: F Volkmar RP, Klin A, Cohen D, editors. Handbook of autism and pervasive developmental disorders. Hoboken,
typical individuals is largely gone [18]. The development
New Jersey: Wiley; 2005. pp. 831–857.
of the somatic map occurs very early in life, which is
Guclu B, Tanidir C, Mukaddes NM, Unal F. Tactile sensitivity of normal and
around the same time that the overgrowth is taking place
autistic children. Somatosens Mot Res 2007; 24:21–33.
Baranek GT. Efficacy of sensory and motor interventions for children with
in the brains of children with autism. Thus, it appears
autism. J Autism Dev Disord 2002; 32:397–422.
likely that the overgrowth in autism has an influence on
Baumgartner C, Doppelbauer A, Deecke L, Barth DS, Zeitlhofer J,
the development of the somatic map. Rather than
Lindinger G, et al. Neuromagnetic investigation of somatotopy of humanhand somatosensory cortex. Exp Brain Res 1991; 87:641–648.
studying a possible outcome of the brain overgrowth as
Castillo EM, Simos PG, Wheless JW, Baumgartner JE, Breier JI, Billingsley RL,
we are doing here with our young adult participants, it
et al. Integrating sensory and motor mapping in a comprehensive MEG
would be more suitable to study the relationship between
protocol: clinical validity and replicability. Neuroimage 2004; 21:973–983.
Suk J, Ribary U, Cappell J, Yamamoto T, Llinas R. Anatomical localization
overgrowth and somatic map abnormality [19]. For this,
revealed by MEG recordings of the human somatosensory system.
one would have to replicate this study with young
Electroencephalogr Clin Neurophysiol 1991; 78:185–196.
children, and observe the development in sensory
Wilson TW, Rojas DC, Reite ML, Teale PD, Rogers SJ. Children andadolescents with autism exhibit reduced MEG steady-state gamma
organization longitudinally both during and after the
responses. Biol Psychiatry 2007; 62:192–197.
overgrowth has occurred. MEG recordings of young
Gage NM, Siegel B, Roberts TP. Cortical auditory system maturational
children 5–6 years old and younger have been successfully
abnormalities in children with autism disorder: an MEG investigation. BrainRes Dev Brain Res 2003; 144:201–209.
carried out [20–22]. Moreover, there are no cognitive
Gage NM, Siegel B, Callen M, Roberts TP. Cortical sound processing in
demands placed on our participants. Our paradigm thus
children with autism disorder: an MEG investigation. Neuroreport 2003;
appears transferable to young children.
Lord C, Rutter M, DiLavore PC, Risi S. Autism Diagnostic ObservationSchedule (ADOS) Manual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services;
The relationship between brain overgrowth and somatic
map development, if one exists, may not be as
Rutter M, Le Couteur A, Lord C. Autism Diagnostic Interview – RevisedManual. Los Angeles: Western Psychological Services; 2003.
straightforward as one might imagine. For example, one
Oldfield RC. The assessment and analysis of handedness: the Edinburgh
is tempted to speculate that the larger brains in people
Inventory. Neuropsychologia 1971; 9:97–113.
with autism imply a stretched out somatic map. This
Wechsler D. Wechsler Abbreviated Scale of Intelligence Manual. San Antonio, Texas: The Psychological Corporation; 1999.
would explain the larger distance between the cortical
Papanicolaou AC, Pazo-Alvarez P, Castillo EM, Billingsley-Marshall RL,
representations of the lip and the thumb in our autism
Breier JI, Swank PR, et al. Functional neuroimaging with MEG: normativelanguage profiles. Neuroimage 2006; 33:326–342.
group. However, even when the overgrowth was taken
Hlustik P, Solodkin A, Gullapalli RP, Noll DC, Small SL. Somatotopy in
into account, the effect, that is, the larger lip–thumb
human primary motor and somatosensory hand representations revisited.
distance in the cortical map in the autism group,
Schaefer M, Heinze HJ, Rotte M. Task-relevant modulation of primary
persisted. Other factors, besides the forces responsible
somatosensory cortex suggests a prefrontal-cortical sensory gating system.
for increasing brain volume, such as those affecting
Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Somatic cortical maps in autism Coskun et al.
Gomez Gonzalez CM, Clark VP, Fan S, Luck SJ, Hillyard SA. Sources of
Fujioka T, Ross B, Kakigi R, Pantev C, Trainor LJ. One year of musical training
attention-sensitive visual event-related potentials. Brain Topogr 1994;
affects development of auditory cortical-evoked fields in young children.
Brain 2006; 129 (Pt 10):2593–2608.
Redcay E, Courchesne E. When is the brain enlarged in autism? A meta-
Paetau R, Hamalainen M, Hari R, Kajola M, Karhu J, Larsen TA, et al.
analysis of all brain size reports. Biol Psychiatry 2005; 58:1–9.
Magnetoencephalographic evaluation of children and adolescents with
Courchesne E, Redcay E, Morgan JT, Kennedy DP. Autism at the
intractable epilepsy. Epilepsia 1994; 35:275–284.
beginning: microstructural and growth abnormalities underlying the
Rojas DC, Maharajh K, Teale PD, Kleman MR, Benkers TL, Carlson JP, et al.
cognitive and behavioral phenotype of autism. Dev Psychopathol 2005;
Development of the 40Hz steady state auditory evoked magnetic field from
ages 5 to 52. Clin Neurophysiol 2006; 117:110–117.
Copyright Lippincott Williams & Wilkins. Unauthorized reproduction of this article is prohibited.
Tag der Bienengesundheit, Auweiler, 24.08.2007 Vortrag 1: Kirsten Traynor: Bienensterben in den USA (CCD – Colony Collapse Disorder) Stipendiatin aus Maryland am Bieneninstitut in Celle In den USA gibt es nur 200.000 Imker mit ca. 1,5 Mio Bienenvölkern (sinkende Tendenz, vor wenigen Jahren noch über 4 Mio. Völker), betreut aber nur von vier wissenschaftlichen Bienenforschungseinr
GEBRAUCHSINFORMATION: INFORMATION FÜR DEN ANWENDER Grippostad® C Kapseln Lesen Sie die gesamte Packungsbeilage sorgfältig durch, denn Um das Risiko einer Überdosierung zu vermeiden, sollte sicher- sie enthält wichtige Informationen für Sie. gestellt werden, dass gleichzeitig angewendete Medikamente kein Dieses Arzneimittel ist ohne Verschreibung erhältlich. Um einen Paracetamol